These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

137 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 36353874)

  • 1. Representational exchange in social learning: Blurring the lines between the ritual and instrumental.
    Vélez N; Wu CM; Cushman FA
    Behav Brain Sci; 2022 Nov; 45():e271. PubMed ID: 36353874
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Imitative flexibility and the development of cultural learning.
    Legare CH; Wen NJ; Herrmann PA; Whitehouse H
    Cognition; 2015 Sep; 142():351-61. PubMed ID: 26083314
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Tradition and invention: The bifocal stance theory of cultural evolution.
    Jagiello R; Heyes C; Whitehouse H
    Behav Brain Sci; 2022 Feb; 45():e249. PubMed ID: 35139964
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Imitation and Innovation: The Dual Engines of Cultural Learning.
    Legare CH; Nielsen M
    Trends Cogn Sci; 2015 Nov; 19(11):688-699. PubMed ID: 26440121
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Preschool children's learning proclivities: When the ritual stance trumps the instrumental stance.
    Wilks M; Kapitány R; Nielsen M
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2016 Sep; 34(3):402-14. PubMed ID: 26918867
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. The early social significance of shared ritual actions.
    Liberman Z; Kinzler KD; Woodward AL
    Cognition; 2018 Feb; 171():42-51. PubMed ID: 29107887
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Can bifocal stance theory explain children's selectivity in active information transmission?
    Bazhydai M; Karadağ D
    Behav Brain Sci; 2022 Nov; 45():e251. PubMed ID: 36353893
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. What is the simplest model that can account for high-fidelity imitation?
    Leibo JZ; Köster R; Vezhnevets AS; Duénez-Guzmán EA; Agapiou JP; Sunehag P
    Behav Brain Sci; 2022 Nov; 45():e261. PubMed ID: 36353886
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. If you presume relevance, you don't need a bifocal lens.
    Altınok N; Tatone D; Király I; Heintz C; Gergely G
    Behav Brain Sci; 2022 Nov; 45():e250. PubMed ID: 36353865
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Considering individual differences and variability is important in the development of the bifocal stance theory.
    Puttre H; Corriveau KH
    Behav Brain Sci; 2022 Nov; 45():e266. PubMed ID: 36353860
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Developmental changes in the influence of conventional and instrumental cues on over-imitation in 3- to 6-year-old children.
    Moraru CA; Gomez JC; McGuigan N
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2016 May; 145():34-47. PubMed ID: 26774258
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Culture is an optometrist: Cultural contexts adjust the prescription of social learning bifocals.
    Clegg JM; Wen NJ; Rawlings B
    Behav Brain Sci; 2022 Nov; 45():e255. PubMed ID: 36353864
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. A cross-cultural comparison of children's imitative flexibility.
    Clegg JM; Legare CH
    Dev Psychol; 2016 Sep; 52(9):1435-44. PubMed ID: 27570982
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Implications of instrumental and ritual stances for traditionalism-threat responsivity relationships.
    Samore T; Fessler DMT
    Behav Brain Sci; 2022 Nov; 45():e267. PubMed ID: 36353878
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Beyond rational imitation: learning arbitrary means actions from communicative demonstrations.
    Király I; Csibra G; Gergely G
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2013 Oct; 116(2):471-86. PubMed ID: 23499323
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. A model's natural group membership affects over-imitation in 6-year-olds.
    Krieger AAR; Aschersleben G; Sommerfeld L; Buttelmann D
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2020 Apr; 192():104783. PubMed ID: 31951928
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Cultural transmission in an ever-changing world: trial-and-error copying may be more robust than precise imitation.
    Truskanov N; Prat Y
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2018 Apr; 373(1743):. PubMed ID: 29440516
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Stick to the script: the effect of witnessing multiple actors on children's imitation.
    Herrmann PA; Legare CH; Harris PL; Whitehouse H
    Cognition; 2013 Dec; 129(3):536-43. PubMed ID: 24045001
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Revisiting an extant framework: Concerns about culture and task generalization.
    Fong FTK; Nielsen M; Legare CH
    Behav Brain Sci; 2022 Nov; 45():e257. PubMed ID: 36353863
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Social learning by imitation in a reptile (Pogona vitticeps).
    Kis A; Huber L; Wilkinson A
    Anim Cogn; 2015 Jan; 18(1):325-31. PubMed ID: 25199480
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.