BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

151 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 36588167)

  • 1. Building representations of the social world: Children extract patterns from social choices to reason about multi-group hierarchies.
    Heck IA; Kushnir T; Kinzler KD
    Dev Sci; 2023 Jul; 26(4):e13366. PubMed ID: 36588167
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Social sampling: Children track social choices to reason about status hierarchies.
    Heck IA; Kushnir T; Kinzler KD
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2021 Aug; 150(8):1673-1687. PubMed ID: 33523688
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. What Could Go Wrong: Adults and Children Calibrate Predictions and Explanations of Others' Actions Based on Relative Reward and Danger.
    Gjata NN; Ullman TD; Spelke ES; Liu S
    Cogn Sci; 2022 Jul; 46(7):e13163. PubMed ID: 35738555
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Learning about causes from people and about people as causes: probabilistic models and social causal reasoning.
    Buchsbaum D; Seiver E; Bridgers S; Gopnik A
    Adv Child Dev Behav; 2012; 43():125-60. PubMed ID: 23205410
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. The choice is yours: Infants' expectations about an agent's future behavior based on taking and receiving actions.
    Eason AE; Doctor D; Chang E; Kushnir T; Sommerville JA
    Dev Psychol; 2018 May; 54(5):829-841. PubMed ID: 29283594
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. The influence of a bystander agent's beliefs on children's and adults' decision-making process.
    Buttelmann F; Buttelmann D
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2017 Jan; 153():126-139. PubMed ID: 27741442
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Children's thinking about group-based social hierarchies.
    Heck IA; Shutts K; Kinzler KD
    Trends Cogn Sci; 2022 Jul; 26(7):593-606. PubMed ID: 35606254
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Young children infer preferences from a single action, but not if it is constrained.
    Pesowski ML; Denison S; Friedman O
    Cognition; 2016 Oct; 155():168-175. PubMed ID: 27416301
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Domain generality and specificity in children's causal inference about ambiguous data.
    Sobel DM; Munro SE
    Dev Psychol; 2009 Mar; 45(2):511-24. PubMed ID: 19271835
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Children's understanding of the costs and rewards underlying rational action.
    Jara-Ettinger J; Gweon H; Tenenbaum JB; Schulz LE
    Cognition; 2015 Jul; 140():14-23. PubMed ID: 25867996
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Do 12.5-month-old infants consider what objects others can see when interpreting their actions?
    Luo Y; Baillargeon R
    Cognition; 2007 Dec; 105(3):489-512. PubMed ID: 17182023
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. "Yay! Yuck!" toddlers use others' emotional responses to reason about hidden objects.
    Smith-Flores AS; Feigenson L
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2022 Sep; 221():105464. PubMed ID: 35644055
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Can 5-month-old infants consider the perspective of a novel eyeless agent? New evidence for early mentalistic reasoning.
    Choi Y; Luo Y; Baillargeon R
    Child Dev; 2022 Mar; 93(2):571-581. PubMed ID: 34766636
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Identifying social partners through indirect prosociality: A computational account.
    Davis I; Carlson R; Dunham Y; Jara-Ettinger J
    Cognition; 2023 Nov; 240():105580. PubMed ID: 37572564
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Visibly constraining an agent modulates observers' automatic false-belief tracking.
    Low J; Edwards K; Butterfill SA
    Sci Rep; 2020 Jul; 10(1):11311. PubMed ID: 32647240
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Children's use of categories and mental states to predict social behavior.
    Chalik L; Rivera C; Rhodes M
    Dev Psychol; 2014 Oct; 50(10):2360-7. PubMed ID: 25151249
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Are category labels primary? Children use similarities to reason about social groups.
    Jordan A; Dunham Y
    Dev Sci; 2021 Mar; 24(2):e13013. PubMed ID: 32627914
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Fulfilment of intention and desire in children's judgements of emotion for sociomoral events.
    Shiverick SM; Moore CF
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2013 Nov; 31(4):395-407. PubMed ID: 24128171
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Infants' selective imitation of a transitive agent and an intransitive agent.
    Luo Y; Pattanakul D; Weng Q; Markson L
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2022 Dec; 224():105517. PubMed ID: 35932639
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Folic acid supplementation and malaria susceptibility and severity among people taking antifolate antimalarial drugs in endemic areas.
    Crider K; Williams J; Qi YP; Gutman J; Yeung L; Mai C; Finkelstain J; Mehta S; Pons-Duran C; Menéndez C; Moraleda C; Rogers L; Daniels K; Green P
    Cochrane Database Syst Rev; 2022 Feb; 2(2022):. PubMed ID: 36321557
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.