These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

120 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 36913792)

  • 61. "Why does rain fall?": children prefer to learn from an informant who uses noncircular explanations.
    Corriveau KH; Kurkul KE
    Child Dev; 2014; 85(5):1827-35. PubMed ID: 24646210
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 62. Social cognition and trust: Exploring the role of theory of mind and hostile attribution bias in children's skepticism of inaccurate informants.
    Palmquist CM; Floersheimer A; Crum K; Ruggiero J
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2022 Mar; 215():105341. PubMed ID: 34906763
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 63. Early testimonial learning: monitoring speech acts and speakers.
    Stephens E; Suarez S; Koenig M
    Adv Child Dev Behav; 2015; 48():151-83. PubMed ID: 25735944
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 64. On the malleability of selective trust.
    Leech KA; Haber AS; Arunachalam S; Kurkul K; Corriveau KH
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2019 Jul; 183():65-74. PubMed ID: 30856418
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 65. Children's socio-moral judgments and behaviors toward peers with and without incarcerated parents.
    Dunlea JP; Goel D; Heiphetz L
    Child Dev; 2022 Sep; 93(5):e515-e530. PubMed ID: 35608230
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 66. Epistemic Trust and Education: Effects of Informant Reliability on Student Learning of Decimal Concepts.
    Durkin K; Shafto P
    Child Dev; 2016; 87(1):154-64. PubMed ID: 26548571
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 67. How do children weigh competence and benevolence when deciding whom to trust?
    Johnston AM; Mills CM; Landrum AR
    Cognition; 2015 Nov; 144():76-90. PubMed ID: 26254218
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 68. Epistemic Vigilance in Early Ontogeny: Children's Use of Nonverbal Behavior to Detect Deception.
    Ghossainy ME; Al-Shawaf L; Woolley JD
    Evol Psychol; 2021; 19(1):1474704920986860. PubMed ID: 33499655
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 69. Children's Judgments of Cultural Expertise: The Influence of Cultural Status and Learning Method.
    Marble KE; Boseovski JJ
    J Genet Psychol; 2019; 180(1):17-30. PubMed ID: 30789083
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 70. Children's Selective Trust in Promises.
    Isella M; Kanngiesser P; Tomasello M
    Child Dev; 2019 Nov; 90(6):e868-e887. PubMed ID: 29959766
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 71. False belief understanding and moral judgment in young children.
    Ochoa KD; Rodini JF; Moses LJ
    Dev Psychol; 2022 Nov; 58(11):2022-2035. PubMed ID: 35816589
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 72. Epistemic trust: modeling children's reasoning about others' knowledge and intent.
    Shafto P; Eaves B; Navarro DJ; Perfors A
    Dev Sci; 2012 May; 15(3):436-47. PubMed ID: 22490183
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 73. Children's moral judgments about psychological harm: Links among harm salience, victims' vulnerability, and child sympathy.
    Yoo HN; Smetana JG
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2019 Dec; 188():104655. PubMed ID: 31430571
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 74. Secondary confessions: the influence (or lack thereof) of incentive size and scientific expert testimony on jurors' perceptions of informant testimony.
    Maeder EM; Pica E
    Law Hum Behav; 2014 Dec; 38(6):560-8. PubMed ID: 25180762
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 75. Does children's moral compass waver under social pressure? Using the conformity paradigm to test preschoolers' moral and social-conventional judgments.
    Kim EB; Chen C; Smetana JG; Greenberger E
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2016 Oct; 150():241-251. PubMed ID: 27367300
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 76. Children's expectations about conventional and moral behaviors of ingroup and outgroup members.
    Liberman Z; Howard LH; Vasquez NM; Woodward AL
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2018 Jan; 165():7-18. PubMed ID: 28404217
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 77. Of Kids and Unicorns: How Rational Is Children's Trust in Testimonial Knowledge?
    Lascaux A
    Cogn Sci; 2020 Mar; 44(3):e12819. PubMed ID: 32090379
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 78. Early tracking of informant accuracy and inaccuracy.
    Corriveau KH; Meints K; Harris PL
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2009 Jun; 27(Pt 2):331-42. PubMed ID: 19998535
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 79. The development of distrust.
    Vanderbilt KE; Liu D; Heyman GD
    Child Dev; 2011; 82(5):1372-80. PubMed ID: 21824130
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 80. Individual Differences in Prototypical Moral and Conventional Judgments and Children's Proactive and Reactive Aggression.
    Jambon M; Smetana JG
    Child Dev; 2018 Jul; 89(4):1343-1359. PubMed ID: 28195420
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 6.