These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

161 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 37282531)

  • 1. An initial but receding altercentric bias in preverbal infants' memory.
    Manea V; Kampis D; Grosse Wiesmann C; Revencu B; Southgate V
    Proc Biol Sci; 2023 Jun; 290(2000):20230738. PubMed ID: 37282531
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Are infants altercentric? The other and the self in early social cognition.
    Southgate V
    Psychol Rev; 2020 Jul; 127(4):505-523. PubMed ID: 31868391
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. The developmental origins of naïve psychology in infancy.
    Poulin-Dubois D; Brooker I; Chow V
    Adv Child Dev Behav; 2009; 37():55-104. PubMed ID: 19673160
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Seeing the World From Others' Perspective: 14-Month-Olds Show Altercentric Modulation Effects by Others' Beliefs.
    Kampis D; Kovács ÁM
    Open Mind (Camb); 2021; 5():189-207. PubMed ID: 36438424
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. The role of redundant verbal labels in 8- and 10-month-olds' working memory.
    Fitch A; Thaker N; Kaldy Z
    Infant Behav Dev; 2021 Aug; 64():101617. PubMed ID: 34339921
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Infants' perseverative search errors are induced by pragmatic misinterpretation.
    Topál J; Gergely G; Miklósi A; Erdohegyi A; Csibra G
    Science; 2008 Sep; 321(5897):1831-4. PubMed ID: 18818358
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Observing others' joint attention increases 9-month-old infants' object encoding.
    Thiele M; Hepach R; Michel C; Haun DBM
    Dev Psychol; 2021 Jun; 57(6):837-850. PubMed ID: 34424003
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Evidence against implicit belief processing in a blindfold task.
    Rothmaler K; Grosse Wiesmann C
    PLoS One; 2023; 18(11):e0294136. PubMed ID: 37956182
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Eye Contact Affects Object Representation in 9-Month-Old Infants.
    Okumura Y; Kobayashi T; Itakura S
    PLoS One; 2016; 11(10):e0165145. PubMed ID: 27776155
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Negativity bias in infants' expectations about agents' dispositions.
    Chae JJK; Song HJ
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2018 Nov; 36(4):620-633. PubMed ID: 29717497
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Infants' search for visible objects: implications for the interpretation of early search errors.
    Sophian C; Yengo L
    J Exp Child Psychol; 1985 Oct; 40(2):260-78. PubMed ID: 4045380
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. How robust are egocentric and altercentric interference effects in social cognition? a test with explicit and implicit versions of a continuous false belief task.
    Haskaraca FN; Proft M; Liszkowski U; Rakoczy H
    Front Psychol; 2023; 14():1142302. PubMed ID: 37492453
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Neural signatures for sustaining object representations attributed to others in preverbal human infants.
    Kampis D; Parise E; Csibra G; Kovács ÁM
    Proc Biol Sci; 2015 Nov; 282(1819):. PubMed ID: 26559949
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. 14-Month-olds anticipate others' actions based on their belief about an object's identity.
    Buttelmann F; Kovács ÁM
    Infancy; 2019 Sep; 24(5):738-751. PubMed ID: 32677281
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Attributing false beliefs about non-obvious properties at 18 months.
    Scott RM; Baillargeon R; Song HJ; Leslie AM
    Cogn Psychol; 2010 Dec; 61(4):366-95. PubMed ID: 21047625
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. The mentalistic basis of core social cognition: experiments in preverbal infants and a computational model.
    Kiley Hamlin J; Ullman T; Tenenbaum J; Goodman N; Baker C
    Dev Sci; 2013 Mar; 16(2):209-226. PubMed ID: 23432831
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Can infants adopt underspecified contents into attributed beliefs? Representational prerequisites of theory of mind.
    Kovács ÁM; Téglás E; Csibra G
    Cognition; 2021 Aug; 213():104640. PubMed ID: 33757642
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Infants' observation of tool-use events over the first year of life.
    Libertus K; Greif ML; Needham AW; Pelphrey K
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2016 Dec; 152():123-135. PubMed ID: 27522041
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Altercentric Cognition: How Others Influence Our Cognitive Processing.
    Kampis D; Southgate V
    Trends Cogn Sci; 2020 Nov; 24(11):945-959. PubMed ID: 32981846
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Learning from picture books: Infants' use of naming information.
    Khu M; Graham SA; Ganea PA
    Front Psychol; 2014; 5():144. PubMed ID: 24611058
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 9.