These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
116 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 37356376)
1. Looking away to see: The acquisition of a search habit away from the saccade direction. Chen C; Lee VG Vision Res; 2023 Oct; 211():108276. PubMed ID: 37356376 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
2. Independence of implicitly guided attention from goal-driven oculomotor control. Chen C; Lee VG Atten Percept Psychophys; 2022 Jul; 84(5):1460-1476. PubMed ID: 35538292 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
3. Implicitly-learned spatial attention is unimpaired in patients with Parkinson's disease. Sisk CA; Twedell EL; Koutstaal W; Cooper SE; Jiang YV Neuropsychologia; 2018 Oct; 119():34-44. PubMed ID: 30063912 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
4. Contribution of peripheral vision to attentional learning. Chen C; Lee VG Atten Percept Psychophys; 2024 Jan; 86(1):95-108. PubMed ID: 37985596 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
5. Acquisition of habitual visual attention and transfer to related tasks. Salovich NA; Remington RW; Jiang YV Psychon Bull Rev; 2018 Jun; 25(3):1052-1058. PubMed ID: 28698989 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
6. Is probabilistic cuing of visual search an inflexible attentional habit? A meta-analytic review. Giménez-Fernández T; Luque D; Shanks DR; Vadillo MA Psychon Bull Rev; 2022 Apr; 29(2):521-529. PubMed ID: 34816390 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
7. The control of saccade trajectories: direction of curvature depends on prior knowledge of target location and saccade latency. Walker R; McSorley E; Haggard P Percept Psychophys; 2006 Jan; 68(1):129-38. PubMed ID: 16617837 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
8. Adaptive deployment of spatial and feature-based attention before saccades. White AL; Rolfs M; Carrasco M Vision Res; 2013 Jun; 85():26-35. PubMed ID: 23147690 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
9. Task specificity of attention training: the case of probability cuing. Jiang YV; Swallow KM; Won BY; Cistera JD; Rosenbaum GM Atten Percept Psychophys; 2015 Jan; 77(1):50-66. PubMed ID: 25113853 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
10. Habit-like attentional bias is unlike goal-driven attentional bias against spatial updating. Hong I; Kim MS Cogn Res Princ Implic; 2022 Jun; 7(1):50. PubMed ID: 35713814 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
11. Aging affects the balance between goal-guided and habitual spatial attention. Twedell EL; Koutstaal W; Jiang YV Psychon Bull Rev; 2017 Aug; 24(4):1135-1141. PubMed ID: 27995541 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
12. Predictive elements in ocular interception and tracking of a moving target by untrained cats. Klam F; Petit J; Grantyn A; Berthoz A Exp Brain Res; 2001 Jul; 139(2):233-47. PubMed ID: 11497066 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
16. Peripheral vision contributes to implicit attentional learning: Findings from the "mouse-eye" paradigm. Chen C; Lee VG Atten Percept Psychophys; 2024 Jun; ():. PubMed ID: 38839714 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
17. Simulated central vision loss does not impair implicit location probability learning when participants search through simple displays. Addleman DA; Lee VG Atten Percept Psychophys; 2022 Aug; 84(6):1901-1912. PubMed ID: 34921336 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
18. Saccadic reaction time in the monkey: advanced preparation of oculomotor programs is primarily responsible for express saccade occurrence. Paré M; Munoz DP J Neurophysiol; 1996 Dec; 76(6):3666-81. PubMed ID: 8985865 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
19. Reward learning biases the direction of saccades. Liao MR; Anderson BA Cognition; 2020 Mar; 196():104145. PubMed ID: 31770659 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
20. Eye-hand coordination during target selection in a pop-out visual search. Song JH; McPeek RM J Neurophysiol; 2009 Nov; 102(5):2681-92. PubMed ID: 19726722 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related] [Next] [New Search]