These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

114 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 37358540)

  • 1. Young children can infer information preferences from goals and recommend appropriate sources to others.
    Chuey A; Lockhart K; Trouche E; Keil F
    Dev Psychol; 2023 Aug; 59(8):1511-1518. PubMed ID: 37358540
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Says who? Children consider informants' sources when deciding whom to believe.
    Aboody R; Yousif SR; Sheskin M; Keil FC
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2022 Oct; 151(10):2481-2493. PubMed ID: 35286115
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Folic acid supplementation and malaria susceptibility and severity among people taking antifolate antimalarial drugs in endemic areas.
    Crider K; Williams J; Qi YP; Gutman J; Yeung L; Mai C; Finkelstain J; Mehta S; Pons-Duran C; Menéndez C; Moraleda C; Rogers L; Daniels K; Green P
    Cochrane Database Syst Rev; 2022 Feb; 2(2022):. PubMed ID: 36321557
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Taking account of others' goals in social information use: Developmental changes in 3- to 7-year-old children.
    Blakey KH; Atkinson M; Rafetseder E; Renner E; Caldwell CA
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2022 Mar; 215():105325. PubMed ID: 34896763
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Emphasizing others' persistence can promote unwarranted social inferences in children and adults.
    Amemiya J; Heyman GD; Walker CM
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2023 Oct; 152(10):2977-2988. PubMed ID: 37199969
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Understanding others' knowledge certainty from inference and information-seeking behaviors in children.
    Huang Z; Hu Q; Shao Y
    Dev Psychol; 2019 Jul; 55(7):1372-1379. PubMed ID: 30945881
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Novelty preferences depend on goals.
    Sehl CG; Tran E; Denison S; Friedman O
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2022 Dec; 29(6):2293-2301. PubMed ID: 35618942
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. When preferences are in the way: Children's predictions of goal-directed behaviors.
    Yang F; Frye D
    Dev Psychol; 2018 Jun; 54(6):1051-1062. PubMed ID: 29251965
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. The odds tell children what people favor.
    Doan T; Stonehouse E; Denison S; Friedman O
    Dev Psychol; 2022 Sep; 58(9):1759-1766. PubMed ID: 35653762
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Children's questions: a mechanism for cognitive development.
    Chouinard MM
    Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 2007; 72(1):vii-ix, 1-112; discussion 113-26. PubMed ID: 17394580
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Toward a Developmental Science of Politics.
    Patterson MM; Bigler RS; Pahlke E; Brown CS; Hayes AR; Ramirez MC; Nelson A
    Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 2019 Sep; 84(3):7-185. PubMed ID: 31503346
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Contingent responsivity in E-books modeled from quality adult-child interactions: Effects on children's learning and attention.
    Eng CM; Tomasic AS; Thiessen ED
    Dev Psychol; 2020 Feb; 56(2):285-297. PubMed ID: 31855013
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. The better part of not knowing: Virtuous ignorance.
    Kominsky JF; Langthorne P; Keil FC
    Dev Psychol; 2016 Jan; 52(1):31-45. PubMed ID: 26479546
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Young children infer preferences from a single action, but not if it is constrained.
    Pesowski ML; Denison S; Friedman O
    Cognition; 2016 Oct; 155():168-175. PubMed ID: 27416301
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. From Exploration to Instruction: Children Learn From Exploration and Tailor Their Demonstrations to Observers' Goals and Competence.
    Gweon H; Schulz L
    Child Dev; 2019 Jan; 90(1):e148-e164. PubMed ID: 29635785
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Functional understanding facilitates learning about tools in human children.
    Hernik M; Csibra G
    Curr Opin Neurobiol; 2009 Feb; 19(1):34-8. PubMed ID: 19477630
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Children employ wealth cues when predicting others' sharing.
    Ahl RE; Duong S; Dunham Y
    Dev Psychol; 2019 Feb; 55(2):303-314. PubMed ID: 30525834
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Should you ask a fisherman or a biologist?: Developmental shifts in ways of clustering knowledge.
    Danovitch JH; Keil FC
    Child Dev; 2004; 75(3):918-31. PubMed ID: 15144494
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. The developmental origins of naïve psychology in infancy.
    Poulin-Dubois D; Brooker I; Chow V
    Adv Child Dev Behav; 2009; 37():55-104. PubMed ID: 19673160
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. 'I bet you know more and are nicer too!': what children infer from others' accuracy.
    Brosseau-Liard PE; Birch SA
    Dev Sci; 2010 Sep; 13(5):772-8. PubMed ID: 20712743
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 6.