These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

122 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 37639235)

  • 21. Grammatical gender and anthropomorphism: "It" depends on the language.
    Mecit A; Lowrey TM; Shrum LJ
    J Pers Soc Psychol; 2022 Sep; 123(3):503-517. PubMed ID: 35311334
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Perceived social isolation and cognition.
    Cacioppo JT; Hawkley LC
    Trends Cogn Sci; 2009 Oct; 13(10):447-54. PubMed ID: 19726219
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. The effects of changes in the referential problem space of infants and toddlers (Homo sapiens): Implications for cross-species comparisons.
    Clark H; Flack ZM; Leavens DA
    J Comp Psychol; 2020 Aug; 134(3):330-340. PubMed ID: 32091227
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. The developmental origins of naïve psychology in infancy.
    Poulin-Dubois D; Brooker I; Chow V
    Adv Child Dev Behav; 2009; 37():55-104. PubMed ID: 19673160
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Do Young Chinese Children Gain Anthropomorphism after Exposure to Personified Touch-Screen and Board Games?
    Li H; Hsueh Y; Wang F; Bai X; Liu T; Zhou L
    Front Psychol; 2017; 8():55. PubMed ID: 28179891
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Better than my past self: Temporal comparison raises children's pride without triggering superiority goals.
    Gürel Ç; Brummelman E; Sedikides C; Overbeek G
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2020 Aug; 149(8):1554-1566. PubMed ID: 31944812
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Spontaneous social tool use in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).
    Schweinfurth MK; DeTroy SE; van Leeuwen EJC; Call J; Haun DBM
    J Comp Psychol; 2018 Nov; 132(4):455-463. PubMed ID: 30451529
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Is the cultural transmission of irrelevant tool actions in adult humans (Homo sapiens) best explained as the result of an evolved conformist bias?
    McGuigan N; Gladstone D; Cook L
    PLoS One; 2012; 7(12):e50863. PubMed ID: 23251395
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Prospective information-seeking in human children (Homo sapiens): When to seek and what to seek.
    Iwasaki S; Kuroshima H; Arahori M; Fujita K
    J Comp Psychol; 2020 Aug; 134(3):341-348. PubMed ID: 32105114
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Better together: Integrative analysis of behavioral gratitude in close relationships using the three-factorial interpersonal emotions (TIE) framework.
    Chang YP; Dwyer PC; Algoe SB
    Emotion; 2022 Dec; 22(8):1739-1754. PubMed ID: 34793186
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Contrasting Social and Cognitive Accounts on Overimitation: The Role of Causal Transparency and Prior Experiences.
    Schleihauf H; Graetz S; Pauen S; Hoehl S
    Child Dev; 2018 May; 89(3):1039-1055. PubMed ID: 28332193
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Why human environments enhance animal capacities to use objects: Evidence from keas (Nestor notabilis) and apes (Gorilla gorilla, Pan paniscus, Pongo abelii, Pongo pygmaeus).
    Cheng K; Byrne RW
    J Comp Psychol; 2018 Nov; 132(4):419-426. PubMed ID: 30024236
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Enhanced memory for negative social information in borderline personality disorder.
    Niedtfeld I; Renkewitz F; Mädebach A; Hillmann K; Kleindienst N; Schmahl C; Schulze L
    J Abnorm Psychol; 2020 Jul; 129(5):480-491. PubMed ID: 32437207
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. On being forgotten: Memory and forgetting serve as signals of interpersonal importance.
    Ray DG; Gomillion S; Pintea AI; Hamlin I
    J Pers Soc Psychol; 2019 Feb; 116(2):259-276. PubMed ID: 30113192
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Social reward, punishment, and prosociality in paranoia.
    Raihani N; Martinez-Gatell D; Bell V; Foulkes L
    J Abnorm Psychol; 2021 Feb; 130(2):177-185. PubMed ID: 33271038
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Is economic risk proneness in young children (Homo sapiens) driven by exploratory behavior? A comparison with capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella).
    Roig A; Meunier H; Poulingue E; Marty A; Thouvarecq R; Rivière J
    J Comp Psychol; 2022 May; 136(2):140-150. PubMed ID: 35389712
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. The cognitive bases of human tool use.
    Vaesen K
    Behav Brain Sci; 2012 Aug; 35(4):203-18. PubMed ID: 22697258
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Direct and indirect reputation formation in nonhuman great apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus) and human children (Homo sapiens).
    Herrmann E; Keupp S; Hare B; Vaish A; Tomasello M
    J Comp Psychol; 2013 Feb; 127(1):63-75. PubMed ID: 22746158
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Relationship Development with Humanoid Social Robots: Applying Interpersonal Theories to Human-Robot Interaction.
    Fox J; Gambino A
    Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw; 2021 May; 24(5):294-299. PubMed ID: 33434097
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. The same with age: Evidence for age-related similarities in interpersonal accuracy.
    Castro VL; Isaacowitz DM
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2019 Sep; 148(9):1517-1537. PubMed ID: 30550339
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.