These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

130 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 37941084)

  • 21. Variability in infant social responsiveness: Age and situational differences in attention-following.
    Tang Y; Triesch J; Deák GO
    Dev Cogn Neurosci; 2023 Oct; 63():101283. PubMed ID: 37586147
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Social perception: How do 6-month-old infants look at pointing gestures?
    Schmitow C; Kochukhova O; Nyström P
    Infant Behav Dev; 2016 Feb; 42():152-6. PubMed ID: 26859864
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Does the speaker's eye gaze facilitate infants' word segmentation from continuous speech? An ERP study.
    Çetinçelik M; Rowland CF; Snijders TM
    Dev Sci; 2024 Mar; 27(2):e13436. PubMed ID: 37551932
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. The temporal dynamics of infants' joint attention: Effects of others' gaze cues and manual actions.
    Boyer TW; Harding SM; Bertenthal BI
    Cognition; 2020 Apr; 197():104151. PubMed ID: 31877403
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Understanding the referential nature of looking: infants' preference for object-directed gaze.
    Senju A; Csibra G; Johnson MH
    Cognition; 2008 Aug; 108(2):303-19. PubMed ID: 18371943
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Social reward anticipation in infants as revealed by event-related potentials.
    Ishikawa M; Itakura S
    Soc Neurosci; 2022 Oct; 17(5):480-489. PubMed ID: 36259467
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. When do infants begin to follow a point?
    Bertenthal BI; Boyer TW; Harding S
    Dev Psychol; 2014 Aug; 50(8):2036-48. PubMed ID: 24911570
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Automated gaze-contingent objects elicit orientation following in 8-month-old infants.
    Deligianni F; Senju A; Gergely G; Csibra G
    Dev Psychol; 2011 Nov; 47(6):1499-503. PubMed ID: 21942669
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Physiological arousal predicts gaze following in infants.
    Ishikawa M; Itakura S
    Proc Biol Sci; 2019 Feb; 286(1896):20182746. PubMed ID: 30963922
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. What are you looking at? Gaze following with and without target objects in ASD and typical development.
    Thorup E; Nyström P; Bölte S; Falck-Ytter T
    Autism; 2022 Oct; 26(7):1668-1680. PubMed ID: 34903076
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Emergence of joint attention: relationships between gaze following, social referencing, imitation, and naming in infancy.
    Slaughter V; McConnell D
    J Genet Psychol; 2003 Mar; 164(1):54-71. PubMed ID: 12693744
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Social context influences infants' ability to extract statistical information from a sequence of gestures.
    Quadrelli E; Monacò S; Turati C; Bulf H
    Infant Behav Dev; 2020 Nov; 61():101506. PubMed ID: 33227679
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Pupillometry reveals communication-induced object expectations in 12- but not 8-month-old infants.
    Pätzold W; Liszkowski U
    Dev Sci; 2019 Nov; 22(6):e12832. PubMed ID: 30942933
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. The development of joint visual attention: a longitudinal study of gaze following during interactions with mothers and strangers.
    Gredebäck G; Fikke L; Melinder A
    Dev Sci; 2010 Nov; 13(6):839-48. PubMed ID: 20977555
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Face detection in 2- to 6-month-old infants is influenced by gaze direction and species.
    Simpson EA; Maylott SE; Mitsven SG; Zeng G; Jakobsen KV
    Dev Sci; 2020 Mar; 23(2):e12902. PubMed ID: 31505079
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. The influence of social and symbolic cues on observers' gaze behaviour.
    Hermens F; Walker R
    Br J Psychol; 2016 Aug; 107(3):484-502. PubMed ID: 26582135
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Selective responding to human ostensive communication is an early developing capacity of domestic dogs.
    Byosiere SE; Mundry R; Range F; Virányi Z
    Dev Sci; 2023 Jul; 26(4):e13361. PubMed ID: 36545915
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Effects of eye gaze cues provided by the caregiver compared to a stranger on infants' object processing.
    Hoehl S; Wahl S; Michel C; Striano T
    Dev Cogn Neurosci; 2012 Jan; 2(1):81-9. PubMed ID: 22682729
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Development of using experimenter-given cues in infant chimpanzees: longitudinal changes in behavior and cognitive development.
    Okamoto-Barth S; Tomonaga M; Tanaka M; Matsuzawa T
    Dev Sci; 2008 Jan; 11(1):98-108. PubMed ID: 18171372
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Communicating to Learn: Infants' Pointing Gestures Result in Optimal Learning.
    Lucca K; Wilbourn MP
    Child Dev; 2018 May; 89(3):941-960. PubMed ID: 28032638
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.