These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

166 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 37956182)

  • 1. Evidence against implicit belief processing in a blindfold task.
    Rothmaler K; Grosse Wiesmann C
    PLoS One; 2023; 18(11):e0294136. PubMed ID: 37956182
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. How robust are egocentric and altercentric interference effects in social cognition? a test with explicit and implicit versions of a continuous false belief task.
    Haskaraca FN; Proft M; Liszkowski U; Rakoczy H
    Front Psychol; 2023; 14():1142302. PubMed ID: 37492453
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Does altercentric interference rely on mentalizing?: Results from two level-1 perspective-taking tasks.
    Marshall J; Gollwitzer A; Santos LR
    PLoS One; 2018; 13(3):e0194101. PubMed ID: 29566019
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Do people automatically track others' beliefs? Evidence from a continuous measure.
    van der Wel RP; Sebanz N; Knoblich G
    Cognition; 2014 Jan; 130(1):128-33. PubMed ID: 24216021
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Task constraints distinguish perspective inferences from perspective use during discourse interpretation in a false belief task.
    Ferguson HJ; Apperly I; Ahmad J; Bindemann M; Cane J
    Cognition; 2015 Jun; 139():50-70. PubMed ID: 25800351
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Brain activity for spontaneous and explicit mentalizing in adults with autism spectrum disorder: An fMRI study.
    Nijhof AD; Bardi L; Brass M; Wiersema JR
    Neuroimage Clin; 2018; 18():475-484. PubMed ID: 29876255
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. The developmental origins of naïve psychology in infancy.
    Poulin-Dubois D; Brooker I; Chow V
    Adv Child Dev Behav; 2009; 37():55-104. PubMed ID: 19673160
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Egocentric bias across mental and non-mental representations in the Sandbox Task.
    Samuel S; Legg EW; Lurz R; Clayton NS
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2018 Nov; 71(11):2395-2410. PubMed ID: 30362406
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Age of avatar modulates the altercentric bias in a visual perspective-taking task: ERP and behavioral evidence.
    Ferguson HJ; Brunsdon VEA; Bradford EEF
    Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci; 2018 Dec; 18(6):1298-1319. PubMed ID: 30242574
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. The influence of a bystander agent's beliefs on children's and adults' decision-making process.
    Buttelmann F; Buttelmann D
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2017 Jan; 153():126-139. PubMed ID: 27741442
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Dissociating visual perspective taking and belief reasoning using a novel integrated paradigm: A preregistered online study.
    Green R; Shaw DJ; Kessler K
    Cognition; 2023 Jun; 235():105397. PubMed ID: 36871396
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Rethinking egocentric bias: A computer mouse-tracking study of adult belief processing.
    O'Connor RJ; Lucas AP; Riggs KJ
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2024 Feb; 153(2):307-327. PubMed ID: 37883019
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Seeing it my way: a case of a selective deficit in inhibiting self-perspective.
    Samson D; Apperly IA; Kathirgamanathan U; Humphreys GW
    Brain; 2005 May; 128(Pt 5):1102-11. PubMed ID: 15774506
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Does social distance modulate adults' egocentric biases when reasoning about false beliefs?
    Farrar BG; Ostojić L
    PLoS One; 2018; 13(6):e0198616. PubMed ID: 29883498
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Seeing the World From Others' Perspective: 14-Month-Olds Show Altercentric Modulation Effects by Others' Beliefs.
    Kampis D; Kovács ÁM
    Open Mind (Camb); 2021; 5():189-207. PubMed ID: 36438424
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. False belief and counterfactual reasoning in a social environment.
    Van Hoeck N; Begtas E; Steen J; Kestemont J; Vandekerckhove M; Van Overwalle F
    Neuroimage; 2014 Apr; 90():315-25. PubMed ID: 24384151
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Great apes use self-experience to anticipate an agent's action in a false-belief test.
    Kano F; Krupenye C; Hirata S; Tomonaga M; Call J
    Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A; 2019 Oct; 116(42):20904-20909. PubMed ID: 31570582
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Level 2 perspective-taking distinguishes automatic and non-automatic belief-tracking.
    Edwards K; Low J
    Cognition; 2019 Dec; 193():104017. PubMed ID: 31271926
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Two sources of evidence on the non-automaticity of true and false belief ascription.
    Back E; Apperly IA
    Cognition; 2010 Apr; 115(1):54-70. PubMed ID: 20060965
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Altercentric interference in level 1 visual perspective taking reflects the ascription of mental states, not submentalizing.
    Furlanetto T; Becchio C; Samson D; Apperly I
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2016 Feb; 42(2):158-63. PubMed ID: 26389611
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 9.