These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

127 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 38007192)

  • 1. Reality monitoring and metacognitive judgments in a false-memory paradigm.
    Ranjan S; Odegaard B
    Neurosci Res; 2024 Apr; 201():3-17. PubMed ID: 38007192
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Lateral posterior parietal activity during source memory judgments of perceived and imagined events.
    King DR; Miller MB
    Neuropsychologia; 2014 Jan; 53():122-36. PubMed ID: 24269856
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Getting at the source of distinctive encoding effects in the DRM paradigm: evidence from signal-detection measures and source judgments.
    Bodner GE; Huff MJ; Lamontagne RW; Azad T
    Memory; 2017 May; 25(5):647-655. PubMed ID: 27387376
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Lateral posterior parietal activity during reality monitoring discriminations of memories of high and low perceptual vividness.
    King DR; Schubert ML; Miller MB
    Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci; 2015 Sep; 15(3):662-79. PubMed ID: 25925490
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Using a Classroom-Based Deese Roediger McDermott Paradigm to Assess the Effects of Imagery on False Memories.
    Oliver MC; Bays RB; Miller C
    J Vis Exp; 2018 Nov; (141):. PubMed ID: 30507920
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Role of memory strength in reality monitoring decisions: evidence from source attribution biases.
    Hoffman HG
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 1997 Mar; 23(2):371-83. PubMed ID: 9080009
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. The Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) Task: A Simple Cognitive Paradigm to Investigate False Memories in the Laboratory.
    Pardilla-Delgado E; Payne JD
    J Vis Exp; 2017 Jan; (119):. PubMed ID: 28190038
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Influence of response bias and internal/external source on lateral posterior parietal successful retrieval activity.
    King DR; Miller MB
    Cortex; 2017 Jun; 91():126-141. PubMed ID: 28499558
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. False memories and the DRM paradigm: effects of imagery, list, and test type.
    Oliver MC; Bays RB; Zabrucky KM
    J Gen Psychol; 2016; 143(1):33-48. PubMed ID: 26786732
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. A registered report testing the effect of sleep on Deese-Roediger-McDermott false memory: greater lure and veridical recall but fewer intrusions after sleep.
    Mak MHC; O'Hagan A; Horner AJ; Gaskell MG
    R Soc Open Sci; 2023 Dec; 10(12):220595. PubMed ID: 38077219
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Confidence ratings do not distinguish imagination from reality.
    Dijkstra N; Mazor M; Fleming SM
    J Vis; 2024 May; 24(5):13. PubMed ID: 38814936
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Social contagion of memory and the role of self-initiated relative judgments.
    Hart KM; Meade ML
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2021 Jan; 212():103189. PubMed ID: 33125984
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Adaptive false memory: Imagining future scenarios increases false memories in the DRM paradigm.
    Dewhurst SA; Anderson RJ; Grace L; van Esch L
    Mem Cognit; 2016 Oct; 44(7):1076-84. PubMed ID: 27173584
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Increased False-Memory Susceptibility After Mindfulness Meditation.
    Wilson BM; Mickes L; Stolarz-Fantino S; Evrard M; Fantino E
    Psychol Sci; 2015 Oct; 26(10):1567-73. PubMed ID: 26341562
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. The Trajectory of Targets and Critical Lures in the Deese/Roediger-McDermott Paradigm: A Systematic Review.
    Coburn PI; Dogra KK; Rai IK; Bernstein DM
    Front Psychol; 2021; 12():718818. PubMed ID: 34925128
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Impaired action self-monitoring and cognitive confidence among ultra-high risk for psychosis and first-episode psychosis patients.
    Gawęda Ł; Li E; Lavoie S; Whitford TJ; Moritz S; Nelson B
    Eur Psychiatry; 2018 Jan; 47():67-75. PubMed ID: 29107832
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Pictorial encoding effects and memory confusions in the Deese-Roediger-McDermott paradigm: evidence for the activation of spontaneous imagery.
    Foley MA; Foy J
    Memory; 2008 Oct; 16(7):712-27. PubMed ID: 18720219
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Source misattributions may increase the accuracy of source judgments.
    Lyle KB; Johnson MK
    Mem Cognit; 2007 Jul; 35(5):1024-33. PubMed ID: 17910186
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Feelings of Contrast at Test Reduce False Memory in the Deese/Roediger-McDermott Paradigm.
    Cadavid S; Beato MS; Suarez M; Albuquerque PB
    Front Psychol; 2021; 12():686390. PubMed ID: 34589019
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. The effect of negative valence on false memory formation in the Deese-Roediger-McDermott paradigm: A preregistered meta-analysis and preregistered replication.
    Wiechert S; Proost D; Simoens E; Ben-Shakhar G; Pertzov Y; Verschuere B
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2024 Mar; 153(3):621-655. PubMed ID: 38059962
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.