These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
3. Mindreading in the balance: adults' mediolateral leaning and anticipatory looking foretell others' action preparation in a false-belief interactive task. Zani G; Butterfill SA; Low J R Soc Open Sci; 2020 Jan; 7(1):191167. PubMed ID: 32218946 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
4. The origins of belief representation: monkeys fail to automatically represent others' beliefs. Martin A; Santos LR Cognition; 2014 Mar; 130(3):300-8. PubMed ID: 24374209 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
5. Do people automatically track others' beliefs? Evidence from a continuous measure. van der Wel RP; Sebanz N; Knoblich G Cognition; 2014 Jan; 130(1):128-33. PubMed ID: 24216021 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
7. Mindreading by body: incorporating mediolateral balance and mouse-tracking measures to examine the motor basis of adults' false-belief tracking. Zani G; Butterfill SA; Low J R Soc Open Sci; 2023 May; 10(5):221212. PubMed ID: 37234504 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
9. The influence of a bystander agent's beliefs on children's and adults' decision-making process. Buttelmann F; Buttelmann D J Exp Child Psychol; 2017 Jan; 153():126-139. PubMed ID: 27741442 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
10. Encoding of others' beliefs without overt instruction. Cohen AS; German TC Cognition; 2009 Jun; 111(3):356-63. PubMed ID: 19376507 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
11. Nonverbal components of Theory of Mind in typical and atypical development. Kampis D; Fogd D; Kovács ÁM Infant Behav Dev; 2017 Aug; 48(Pt A):54-62. PubMed ID: 27856031 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
12. Perceptual Access Reasoning (PAR) in Developing a Representational Theory of Mind. Fabricius WV; Gonzales CR; Pesch A; Weimer AA; Pugliese J; Carroll K; Bolnick RR; Kupfer AS; Eisenberg N; Spinrad TL Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 2021 Sep; 86(3):7-154. PubMed ID: 34580875 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
13. Deconstructing Theory-of-Mind Impairment in High-Functioning Adults with Autism. Rosenthal IA; Hutcherson CA; Adolphs R; Stanley DA Curr Biol; 2019 Feb; 29(3):513-519.e6. PubMed ID: 30686740 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
14. Intensionality is more cognitively demanding than false belief in adults: evidence from a non-inferential sentence-reading task. Yeung EKL; Cheung H Sci Rep; 2024 Jan; 14(1):561. PubMed ID: 38177291 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
15. Pragmatic development explains the Theory-of-Mind Scale. Westra E; Carruthers P Cognition; 2017 Jan; 158():165-176. PubMed ID: 27835787 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
16. Can infants adopt underspecified contents into attributed beliefs? Representational prerequisites of theory of mind. Kovács ÁM; Téglás E; Csibra G Cognition; 2021 Aug; 213():104640. PubMed ID: 33757642 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
17. The social sense: susceptibility to others' beliefs in human infants and adults. Kovács ÁM; Téglás E; Endress AD Science; 2010 Dec; 330(6012):1830-4. PubMed ID: 21205671 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
18. Efficient belief tracking in adults: The role of task instruction, low-level associative processes and dispositional social functioning. Meert G; Wang J; Samson D Cognition; 2017 Nov; 168():91-98. PubMed ID: 28666214 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
19. Being Deceived: Information Asymmetry in Second-Order False Belief Tasks. Braüner T; Blackburn P; Polyanskaya I Top Cogn Sci; 2020 Apr; 12(2):504-534. PubMed ID: 31401814 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
20. How do you know that? Automatic belief inferences in passing conversation. Rubio-Fernández P; Mollica F; Oraa Ali M; Gibson E Cognition; 2019 Dec; 193():104011. PubMed ID: 31255905 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related] [Next] [New Search]