These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

146 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 38223228)

  • 1. Response-Category Conflict and Control Mode Determine Memory Performance for Distractors in a Flanker Paradigm.
    Muhmenthaler MC; Meier B
    J Cogn; 2024; 7(1):9. PubMed ID: 38223228
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Response-category conflict improves target memory in a flanker paradigm.
    Muhmenthaler MC; Meier B
    Memory; 2022 Mar; 30(3):309-316. PubMed ID: 34877901
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Orchestrating Proactive and Reactive Mechanisms for Filtering Distracting Information: Brain-Behavior Relationships Revealed by a Mixed-Design fMRI Study.
    Marini F; Demeter E; Roberts KC; Chelazzi L; Woldorff MG
    J Neurosci; 2016 Jan; 36(3):988-1000. PubMed ID: 26791226
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. A bimodal extension of the Eriksen flanker task.
    Ulrich R; Prislan L; Miller J
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2021 Feb; 83(2):790-799. PubMed ID: 33179215
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Neural correlates of conflict control on facial expressions with a flanker paradigm.
    Liu T; Xiao T; Shi JN
    PLoS One; 2013; 8(7):e69683. PubMed ID: 23894521
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Flanker task with equiprobable congruent and incongruent conditions does not elicit the conflict N2.
    Kałamała P; Szewczyk J; Senderecka M; Wodniecka Z
    Psychophysiology; 2018 Feb; 55(2):. PubMed ID: 28845513
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Effects of working memory load on visual selective attention: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence.
    Pratt N; Willoughby A; Swick D
    Front Hum Neurosci; 2011; 5():57. PubMed ID: 21716633
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Goal-driven selective attention in patients with right hemisphere lesions: how intact is the ipsilesional field?
    Snow JC; Mattingley JB
    Brain; 2006 Jan; 129(Pt 1):168-81. PubMed ID: 16317021
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Ad-hoc and context-dependent adjustments of selective attention in conflict control: an ERP study with visual probes.
    Nigbur R; Schneider J; Sommer W; Dimigen O; Stürmer B
    Neuroimage; 2015 Feb; 107():76-84. PubMed ID: 25482266
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. The influence of reward in the Simon task: Differences and similarities to the Stroop and Eriksen flanker tasks.
    Mittelstädt V; Ulrich R; König J; Hofbauer K; Mackenzie IG
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2023 Apr; 85(3):949-959. PubMed ID: 36316615
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. When the ignored gets bound: sequential effects in the flanker task.
    Davelaar EJ
    Front Psychol; 2012; 3():552. PubMed ID: 23293616
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Visuomotor Correlates of Conflict Expectation in the Context of Motor Decisions.
    Derosiere G; Klein PA; Nozaradan S; Zénon A; Mouraux A; Duque J
    J Neurosci; 2018 Oct; 38(44):9486-9504. PubMed ID: 30201772
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Quantitative relation between conflict and response inhibition in the Flanker task.
    Takezawa T; Miyatani M
    Psychol Rep; 2005 Oct; 97(2):515-26. PubMed ID: 16342579
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Gender differences on the semantic flanker task using transposed-letter target words.
    Judge J; Taylor PJ
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2012; 65(10):2008-17. PubMed ID: 22524791
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Different impact of task switching and response-category conflict on subsequent memory.
    Muhmenthaler MC; Meier B
    Psychol Res; 2021 Mar; 85(2):679-696. PubMed ID: 31802223
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Sequential conflict resolution under multiple concurrent conflicts: An ERP study.
    Rey-Mermet A; Gade M; Steinhauser M
    Neuroimage; 2019 Mar; 188():411-418. PubMed ID: 30562575
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Flanker tasks based on congruency manipulation are biased measures of selective attention in perceptual load studies.
    Li Z; Lou J
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2019 Aug; 81(6):1836-1845. PubMed ID: 30993656
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Cognitive conflict increases processing of negative, task-irrelevant stimuli.
    Ligeza TS; Wyczesany M
    Int J Psychophysiol; 2017 Oct; 120():126-135. PubMed ID: 28757233
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Occipital alpha power reveals fast attentional inhibition of incongruent distractors.
    Janssens C; De Loof E; Boehler CN; Pourtois G; Verguts T
    Psychophysiology; 2018 Mar; 55(3):. PubMed ID: 28929499
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Conditional accuracy in response interference tasks: Evidence from the Eriksen flanker task and the spatial conflict task.
    Stins JF; Polderman JC; Boomsma DI; de Geus EJ
    Adv Cogn Psychol; 2008 Jul; 3(3):409-17. PubMed ID: 20517524
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.