These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

120 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 38864326)

  • 41. Effects of breed group and development on dogs' willingness to follow a human misleading advice.
    Barnard S; Passalacqua C; Pelosi A; Valsecchi P; Prato-Previde E
    Anim Cogn; 2019 Sep; 22(5):757-768. PubMed ID: 31161363
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 42. African elephants (Loxodonta africana) recognize visual attention from face and body orientation.
    Smet AF; Byrne RW
    Biol Lett; 2014 Jul; 10(7):. PubMed ID: 25013015
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 43. Can dogs use vocal intonation as a social referencing cue in an object choice task?
    Colbert-White EN; Tullis A; Andresen DR; Parker KM; Patterson KE
    Anim Cogn; 2018 Mar; 21(2):253-265. PubMed ID: 29442250
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 44. Infants interpret ambiguous requests for absent objects.
    Saylor MM; Ganea P
    Dev Psychol; 2007 May; 43(3):696-704. PubMed ID: 17484581
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 45. Ravens, Corvus corax, follow gaze direction of humans around obstacles.
    Bugnyar T; Stöwe M; Heinrich B
    Proc Biol Sci; 2004 Jul; 271(1546):1331-6. PubMed ID: 15306330
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 46. Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use a physical marker to locate hidden food.
    Riedel J; Buttelmann D; Call J; Tomasello M
    Anim Cogn; 2006 Jan; 9(1):27-35. PubMed ID: 15846526
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 47. Deaf children's use of clear visual cues in mindreading.
    Hao J; Su Y
    Res Dev Disabil; 2014 Nov; 35(11):2849-57. PubMed ID: 25104224
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 48. People represent their own mental states more distinctly than those of others.
    Thornton MA; Weaverdyck ME; Mildner JN; Tamir DI
    Nat Commun; 2019 May; 10(1):2117. PubMed ID: 31073156
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 49. Can infants adopt underspecified contents into attributed beliefs? Representational prerequisites of theory of mind.
    Kovács ÁM; Téglás E; Csibra G
    Cognition; 2021 Aug; 213():104640. PubMed ID: 33757642
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 50. Rhesus monkeys fail to use gaze direction as an experimenter-given cue in an object-choice task.
    Anderson JR; Montant M; Schmitt D
    Behav Processes; 1996 Aug; 37(1):47-55. PubMed ID: 24897158
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 51. Self-experience as a mechanism for learning about others: a training study in social cognition.
    Meltzoff AN; Brooks R
    Dev Psychol; 2008 Sep; 44(5):1257-65. PubMed ID: 18793060
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 52. Age of avatar modulates the altercentric bias in a visual perspective-taking task: ERP and behavioral evidence.
    Ferguson HJ; Brunsdon VEA; Bradford EEF
    Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci; 2018 Dec; 18(6):1298-1319. PubMed ID: 30242574
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 53. Apes' use of iconic cues in the object-choice task.
    Herrmann E; Melis AP; Tomasello M
    Anim Cogn; 2006 Apr; 9(2):118-30. PubMed ID: 16395566
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 54. Dogs follow human misleading suggestions more often when the informant has a false belief.
    Lonardo L; Völter CJ; Lamm C; Huber L
    Proc Biol Sci; 2021 Jul; 288(1955):20210906. PubMed ID: 34284633
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 55. Do non-human primates really represent others' ignorance? A test of the awareness relations hypothesis.
    Horschler DJ; Santos LR; MacLean EL
    Cognition; 2019 Sep; 190():72-80. PubMed ID: 31026672
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 56. The closing of the theory of mind: A critique of perspective-taking.
    Cole GG; Millett AC
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2019 Dec; 26(6):1787-1802. PubMed ID: 31515733
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 57. Effect of training and familiarity on responsiveness to human cues in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris).
    Cunningham CL; Ramos MF
    Anim Cogn; 2014 May; 17(3):805-14. PubMed ID: 24318516
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 58. Tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) show understanding of human attentional states when requesting food held by a human.
    Hattori Y; Kuroshima H; Fujita K
    Anim Cogn; 2010 Jan; 13(1):87-92. PubMed ID: 19517147
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 59. Does the A-not-B error in adult pet dogs indicate sensitivity to human communication?
    Kis A; Topál J; Gácsi M; Range F; Huber L; Miklósi A; Virányi Z
    Anim Cogn; 2012 Jul; 15(4):737-43. PubMed ID: 22460627
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 60. Prelinguistic human infants and great apes show different communicative strategies in a triadic request situation.
    Gretscher H; Tempelmann S; Haun DB; Liebal K; Kaminski J
    PLoS One; 2017; 12(4):e0175227. PubMed ID: 28384300
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 6.