These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
144 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 4075867)
1. Developmental changes in the understanding of temporal sequence. Fivush R; Mandler JM Child Dev; 1985 Dec; 56(6):1437-46. PubMed ID: 4075867 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
2. Children's representations of the pattern of daily activities. Friedman WJ Child Dev; 1990 Oct; 61(5):1399-412. PubMed ID: 2245733 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
3. Searching and planning: young children's reasoning about past and future event sequences. McColgan KL; McCormack T Child Dev; 2008; 79(5):1477-97. PubMed ID: 18826537 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
4. Contribution of the priming paradigm to the understanding of the conceptual developmental shift from 5 to 9 years of age. Perraudin S; Mounoud P Dev Sci; 2009 Nov; 12(6):956-77. PubMed ID: 19840051 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
5. The salience of temporal cues in the developing structure of event knowledge. Thompson LA; Gomez RL; Schvaneveldt RW Am J Psychol; 2000; 113(4):591-619. PubMed ID: 11131744 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
7. Knowledge, concepts, and inferences in childhood. Farrar MJ; Raney GE; Boyer ME Child Dev; 1992 Jun; 63(3):673-91. PubMed ID: 1600830 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
8. Duration, distance, and speed judgments of two moving objects by 4- to 11-year olds. Matsuda F J Exp Child Psychol; 1996 Nov; 63(2):286-311. PubMed ID: 8923748 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
9. Young children's comprehension of montage. Smith R; Anderson DR; Fischer C Child Dev; 1985 Aug; 56(4):962-71. PubMed ID: 4042754 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
10. Flankers facilitate 3-year-olds' performance in a card-sorting task. Jordan PL; Morton JB Dev Psychol; 2008 Jan; 44(1):265-74. PubMed ID: 18194025 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
11. The development of forward search planning in preschoolers. Fabricius WV Child Dev; 1988 Dec; 59(6):1473-88. PubMed ID: 3208559 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
12. When even arbitrary order becomes important: developments in reliable temporal sequencing of arbitrarily ordered events. Bauer PJ; Hertsgaard LA; Dropik P; Daly BP Memory; 1998 Mar; 6(2):165-98. PubMed ID: 9640427 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
13. What young chimpanzees know about seeing. Povinelli DJ; Eddy TJ Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 1996; 61(3):i-vi, 1-152; discussion 153-91. PubMed ID: 8795292 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
15. How do symbols affect 3- to 4-year-olds' executive function? Evidence from a reverse-contingency task. Apperly IA; Carroll DJ Dev Sci; 2009 Nov; 12(6):1070-82. PubMed ID: 19840061 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
16. Blickets and babies: the development of causal reasoning in toddlers and infants. Sobel DM; Kirkham NZ Dev Psychol; 2006 Nov; 42(6):1103-15. PubMed ID: 17087545 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
17. Preschool children's ability to visually represent relations. Koerber S; Sodian B Dev Sci; 2008 May; 11(3):390-5. PubMed ID: 18466372 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
18. Shedding new light on an old problem: The estimation of shadow sizes in children and adults. Ebersbach M; Resing WC J Exp Child Psychol; 2007 Aug; 97(4):265-85. PubMed ID: 17512532 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
19. Children's reasoning about the causal significance of the temporal order of events. McCormack T; Hoerl C Dev Psychol; 2005 Jan; 41(1):54-63. PubMed ID: 15656737 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
20. Children's questions: a mechanism for cognitive development. Chouinard MM Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 2007; 72(1):vii-ix, 1-112; discussion 113-26. PubMed ID: 17394580 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related] [Next] [New Search]