These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
127 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 7146239)
1. Analysis of the components of disparity in Wilde's dot-row phenomenon. van der Meer HC Psychol Res; 1982; 44(2):127-34. PubMed ID: 7146239 [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
2. Disparity-processing in Wilde's dot-row phenomenon. van der Meer HC Psychol Res; 1982; 44(2):105-17. PubMed ID: 7146237 [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
3. Wilde's dot-row phenomenon as a special case of aniseikonia. van der Meer HC Psychol Res; 1982; 44(2):119-26. PubMed ID: 7146238 [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
5. A stereo illusion induced by binocularly presented gratings: effects of number of eyes stimulated, spatial frequency, orientation, field size, and viewing distance. Odom JV; Chao GM Percept Psychophys; 1987 Aug; 42(2):140-9. PubMed ID: 3627934 [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
6. Depth inversion despite stereopsis: the appearance of random-dot stereograms on surfaces seen in reverse perspective. Yellott JI; Kaiwi JL Perception; 1979; 8(2):135-42. PubMed ID: 471677 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
7. Depth effect from a rotating line of constant length. Zanforlin M; Vallortigara G Percept Psychophys; 1988 Nov; 44(5):493-9. PubMed ID: 3226900 [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
8. Pseudoscopic viewing: transfer and persistence of reversed depth relations from the viewing of photographs to the real scene. Kalaugher PG Perception; 1987; 16(3):359-74. PubMed ID: 3432031 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
10. Blur patterns: a comment. Prazdny K Percept Psychophys; 1982 Feb; 31(2):190-1. PubMed ID: 7079100 [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
11. How does adaptation to disparity affect the perception of reversible figures? Harris JP Am J Psychol; 1980 Sep; 93(3):445-57. PubMed ID: 7212126 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
13. Structure from stereo by associative learning of the constraints. O'Toole AJ Perception; 1989; 18(6):767-82. PubMed ID: 2628927 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
15. Pictorial depth and the Poggendorff illusion. Parks TE; Hui LM Percept Psychophys; 1989 Nov; 46(5):465-8. PubMed ID: 2813031 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
16. Illusion reversal rate as a function of subjective depth. Borresen CR J Gen Psychol; 1990 Jan; 117(1):39-46. PubMed ID: 2313278 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
17. The role of apparent depth and context in the perception of the Ponzo illusion. Schiffman HR; Thompson JG Perception; 1978; 7(1):47-50. PubMed ID: 628580 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
18. Monocular stereopsis with and without head movement. Ono H; Steinbach MJ Percept Psychophys; 1990 Aug; 48(2):179-87. PubMed ID: 2385492 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
19. Dot displacements can be deceptive: a reply to Hartley. Wenderoth P Percept Psychophys; 1980 Apr; 27(4):368-9. PubMed ID: 7383822 [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
20. Illusory contours from pictorially three-dimensional inducing elements. Parks TE; Rock I Perception; 1990; 19(1):119-21. PubMed ID: 2336327 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related] [Next] [New Search]