These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

106 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 7147734)

  • 1. The induced effect: arguments against the theory of Arditi, Kaufman and Movshon (1981).
    Mayhew EW; Frisby JP
    Vision Res; 1982; 22(9):1225-8. PubMed ID: 7147734
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. The effect of spatial frequency on colour selectivity in the tilt illusion.
    Lovegrove W; Badcock D
    Vision Res; 1981; 21(8):1235-7. PubMed ID: 7314506
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. [Interpretation of neurophysiologic visual illusions].
    Fugulyan G; Rajka V
    Rev Chir Oncol Radiol O R L Oftalmol Stomatol Ser Oftalmol; 1985; 29(4):257-67. PubMed ID: 2939497
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Absence of accommodation during perceptual reversal of Necker cubes.
    Ellis SR; Wong JH; Stark L
    Vision Res; 1979; 19(8):953-5. PubMed ID: 516469
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Variations in apparent spatial frequency with stimulus orientation: I. Incidence of the effect in the general population.
    Bowker DO
    Percept Psychophys; 1981 Jun; 29(6):563-7. PubMed ID: 7279584
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. [Correlation between individual features of the functional asymmetry of the cerebral hemispheres and pilot success].
    Giurdzhian AA; Fedoruk AG
    Kosm Biol Aviakosm Med; 1982; 16(1):43-5. PubMed ID: 7062698
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. The tilt illusion as a function of the relative and absolute lengths of test and inducing lines.
    Wenderoth P; O'Connor T; Johnson M
    Percept Psychophys; 1986 May; 39(5):339-45. PubMed ID: 3737365
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Perceived size of letters depends on inter-letter spacing: a new visual illusion.
    Skottun BC; Freeman RD
    Vision Res; 1983; 23(1):111-2. PubMed ID: 6868375
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Variations in apparent spatial frequency with stimulus orientation: II. Matching data collected under normal and interferometric viewing conditions.
    Bowker DO
    Percept Psychophys; 1981 Jun; 29(6):568-77. PubMed ID: 7279585
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Apparent movement in the human visual field.
    Choudhury BP; Carvill SC
    Physiol Behav; 1983 Jan; 30(1):73-6. PubMed ID: 6836047
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Exorcising the double-nail confusion: a reply to Ono.
    Krol JD; van de Grind WA
    Perception; 1984; 13(6):759-64. PubMed ID: 6543956
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Processing of visual illusions in the frequency and spatial domains.
    Becker MF; Knopp J
    Percept Psychophys; 1978 Jun; 23(6):521-6. PubMed ID: 683836
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Eye movements and illusions of alignment.
    Findlay JM; Hotopf WH
    Perception; 1985; 14(4):387-91. PubMed ID: 3834383
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Exorcising the double-nail illusion: giving up the ghost.
    Ono H
    Perception; 1984; 13(6):753-8. PubMed ID: 6543955
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Effects of orientation-selective adaptation on the Zöllner illusion.
    Morikawa K
    Perception; 1987; 16(4):473-83. PubMed ID: 3444730
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Size and position are incongruous: measurements on the Müller-Lyer figure.
    Gillam B; Chambers D
    Percept Psychophys; 1985 Jun; 37(6):549-56. PubMed ID: 4059013
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. An algorithm that generates a large number of geometric visual illusions.
    Ninio J
    J Theor Biol; 1979 Jul; 79(2):167-201. PubMed ID: 513807
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. The missing fundamental illusion: variation of spatio-temporal characteristics with dark adaptation.
    Sullivan GD; Georgeson MA
    Vision Res; 1977; 17(8):977-81. PubMed ID: 595405
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Cyclopean stimulation can influence sensations of self-motion in normal and stereoblind subjects.
    Wolfe JM; Held R
    Percept Psychophys; 1980 Aug; 28(2):139-42. PubMed ID: 7432987
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Some proprioceptive influences on the spatial displacement component of the oculogyral illusion.
    Evanoff JN; Lackner JR
    Percept Psychophys; 1988 Jun; 43(6):526-30. PubMed ID: 3399350
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 6.