These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

123 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 7169709)

  • 1. [Filled-duration illusion as a function of visual stimulus tempo].
    Jingu H
    Shinrigaku Kenkyu; 1982 Dec; 53(5):296-9. PubMed ID: 7169709
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. A further investigation of the filled-duration illusion with a comparison between children and adults.
    Droit-Volet S
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2008 Jul; 34(3):400-14. PubMed ID: 18665722
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Differences in duration discrimination of filled and empty auditory intervals as a function of base duration.
    Rammsayer TH
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2010 Aug; 72(6):1591-600. PubMed ID: 20675803
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Filled and empty motor reproductions of filled and empty intervals: Is there also a filled - reproduction illusion?
    Bratzke D; Birngruber T; Durst M; Schröter H
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2017 Oct; 79(7):2143-2152. PubMed ID: 28695540
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. The occurrence of the filled duration illusion: a comparison of the method of adjustment with the method of magnitude estimation.
    Hasuo E; Nakajima Y; Tomimatsu E; Grondin S; Ueda K
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2014 Mar; 147():111-21. PubMed ID: 24210181
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. The role of number and familiarity of stimuli in the perception of brief temporal intervals.
    Schiffman HR; Bobko DJ
    Am J Psychol; 1977 Mar; 90(1):85-93. PubMed ID: 871180
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Time estimation of visually presented increment and decrement stimuli: the filled-duration illusion.
    Marinova J; Mitov D; Mitrani L
    Acta Physiol Pharmacol Bulg; 1980; 6(1):26-33. PubMed ID: 7405591
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Space and time in visual context.
    Schwartz O; Hsu A; Dayan P
    Nat Rev Neurosci; 2007 Jul; 8(7):522-35. PubMed ID: 17585305
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. What is common to brain activity evoked by the perception of visual and auditory filled durations? A study with MEG and EEG co-recordings.
    N'Diaye K; Ragot R; Garnero L; Pouthas V
    Brain Res Cogn Brain Res; 2004 Oct; 21(2):250-68. PubMed ID: 15464356
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Recalibration of temporal order perception by exposure to audio-visual asynchrony.
    Vroomen J; Keetels M; de Gelder B; Bertelson P
    Brain Res Cogn Brain Res; 2004 Dec; 22(1):32-5. PubMed ID: 15561498
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Factors influencing audiovisual fission and fusion illusions.
    Andersen TS; Tiippana K; Sams M
    Brain Res Cogn Brain Res; 2004 Nov; 21(3):301-8. PubMed ID: 15511646
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Internal clock processes and the filled-duration illusion.
    Wearden JH; Norton R; Martin S; Montford-Bebb O
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2007 Jun; 33(3):716-29. PubMed ID: 17563232
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Reduction of stimulus visibility compresses apparent time intervals.
    Terao M; Watanabe J; Yagi A; Nishida S
    Nat Neurosci; 2008 May; 11(5):541-2. PubMed ID: 18408716
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Time course of auditory masker effects: tapping the locus of audiovisual integration?
    Steenken R; Diederich A; Colonius H
    Neurosci Lett; 2008 Apr; 435(1):78-83. PubMed ID: 18355963
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Discrimination of empty and filled intervals marked by auditory signals with different durations and directions of intensity change.
    Chen L
    Psych J; 2019 Jun; 8(2):187-202. PubMed ID: 30666805
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Individual differences in sensory integration predict differences in time perception and individual levels of schizotypy.
    Fenner B; Cooper N; Romei V; Hughes G
    Conscious Cogn; 2020 Sep; 84():102979. PubMed ID: 32702601
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Sensory effects on judgments of short time-intervals.
    Grondin S; Meilleur-Wells G; Ouellette C; Macar F
    Psychol Res; 1998; 61(4):261-8. PubMed ID: 9870293
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. The perception of empty and filled time intervals by rats.
    Santi A; Miki A; Hornyak S; Eidse J
    Behav Processes; 2005 Nov; 70(3):247-63. PubMed ID: 16112508
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. The effect of congenital deafness on duration judgment.
    Kowalska J; Szelag E
    J Child Psychol Psychiatry; 2006 Sep; 47(9):946-53. PubMed ID: 16930389
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Asymmetric cross-modal effects in time perception.
    Chen KM; Yeh SL
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2009 Mar; 130(3):225-34. PubMed ID: 19195633
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.