These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
2. THE EFFECT OF A SINGLE MONOCULAR DEPTH CUE DEFICIT ON RETARDATES' PERCEPTION OF THE ROTATING CUBE ILLUSION. SPITZ HH Am J Ment Defic; 1965 Mar; 69():703-11. PubMed ID: 14286832 [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
3. Age effects on the perception of motion illusions. Billino J; Hamburger K; Gegenfurtner KR Perception; 2009; 38(4):508-21. PubMed ID: 19522320 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
4. A structural analysis of stability in the perceived form of objects rotating in depth. Rankin NO; Yablick GS Psychol Res; 1976 Oct; 39(1):51-65. PubMed ID: 1005628 [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
5. The case for misapplied constancy scaling: depth associations elicited by illusion configurations. Ward LM; Porac C; Coren S; Girgus JS Am J Psychol; 1977 Dec; 90(4):609-20. PubMed ID: 610448 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
6. New illusions of sliding motion in depth. Pinna B; Spillmann L Perception; 2005; 34(12):1441-58. PubMed ID: 16457166 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
7. A dynamic Ponzo illusion: induced direction of rotation in depth. Hershberger W; Laughlin N; Nitschke W Perception; 1976; 5(2):197-202. PubMed ID: 951168 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
8. Motion from occlusion. Engel SA; Remus DA; Sainath R J Vis; 2006 May; 6(5):649-52. PubMed ID: 16881795 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
9. Field dependency and perceptual cues in perception of two trapezoid illusions. Zenhausern R; Renna M Percept Mot Skills; 1976 Dec; 43(3 Pt 2):1043-9. PubMed ID: 1012885 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
10. Illusory three-dimensional rotation of horizontal lines: a new motion-depth illusion. Ito H; Kawabata E Perception; 1998; 27(10):1203-7. PubMed ID: 10505198 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
11. Simulation of an object rotating in depth: constant and reversed projection ratios. Hershberger WA; Carpenter DL; Starzec J; Laughlin NK J Exp Psychol; 1974 Nov; 103(5):844-53. PubMed ID: 4443761 [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
12. Motion parallax as an independent cue for depth perception. Rogers B; Graham M Perception; 1979; 8(2):125-34. PubMed ID: 471676 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
13. Effects of visual-field inversions on the reverse-perspective illusion. Cook ND; Hayashi T; Amemiya T; Suzuki K; Leumann L Perception; 2002; 31(9):1147-51. PubMed ID: 12375877 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
14. Depth cues and constancy scaling in the horizontal-vertical illusion: the bisection error. Girgus JS; Coren S Can J Psychol; 1975 Mar; 29(1):59-65. PubMed ID: 1139456 [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
15. The Ames window illusion: perception of illusory motion by human infants. Oross S; Francis E; Mauk D; Fox R J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 1987 Nov; 13(4):609-13. PubMed ID: 2965753 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
16. The Ponzo illusion without suggested depth. Reardon ME; Parks TE Am J Psychol; 1983; 96(1):107-12. PubMed ID: 6859345 [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
17. The Ponzo illusion in pictures with and without suggested depth. Newman CV; Newman BM Am J Psychol; 1974 Sep; 87(3):511-6. PubMed ID: 4455099 [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
18. Illusion reversal rate as a function of subjective depth. Borresen CR J Gen Psychol; 1990 Jan; 117(1):39-46. PubMed ID: 2313278 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
19. Monocular stereopsis with and without head movement. Ono H; Steinbach MJ Percept Psychophys; 1990 Aug; 48(2):179-87. PubMed ID: 2385492 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
20. Apparent reversals as a function of perceiver group, distance, and geometric shape. Oreel TN; Munz DC Percept Mot Skills; 1978 Oct; 47(2):417-8. PubMed ID: 724380 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related] [Next] [New Search]