These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

133 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 7660592)

  • 1. Visual attention mechanisms show a center-surround organization.
    Steinman BA; Steinman SB; Lehmkuhle S
    Vision Res; 1995 Jul; 35(13):1859-69. PubMed ID: 7660592
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Transient attention degrades perceived apparent motion.
    Yeshurun Y; Hein E
    Perception; 2011; 40(8):905-18. PubMed ID: 22132506
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Perception of static eye gaze direction facilitates subsequent early visual processing.
    Schuller AM; Rossion B
    Clin Neurophysiol; 2004 May; 115(5):1161-8. PubMed ID: 15066541
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Reflexive gaze orienting induces the line-motion illusion.
    Bavelier D; Schneider KA; Monacelli A
    Vision Res; 2002 Nov; 42(26):2817-27. PubMed ID: 12450507
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Attentional shift by gaze is triggered without awareness.
    Sato W; Okada T; Toichi M
    Exp Brain Res; 2007 Oct; 183(1):87-94. PubMed ID: 17624520
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Transient visual attention is dominated by the magnocellular stream.
    Steinman BA; Steinman SB; Lehmkuhle S
    Vision Res; 1997 Jan; 37(1):17-23. PubMed ID: 9068828
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Global induced motion and visual stability in an optic flow illusion.
    Pack C; Mingolla E
    Vision Res; 1998 Oct; 38(20):3083-93. PubMed ID: 9893817
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Microsaccade directions do not predict directionality of illusory brightness changes of overlapping transparent surfaces.
    Tse PU; Caplovitz GP; Hsieh PJ
    Vision Res; 2006 Oct; 46(22):3823-30. PubMed ID: 16934310
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Motion thresholds of briefly visible stimuli increase asymmetrically with age.
    Wertheim AH; Bekkering H
    Vision Res; 1992 Dec; 32(12):2379-84. PubMed ID: 1288014
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. An illusion of relative motion dependent upon spatial frequency and orientation.
    Hine TJ; Cook M; Rogers GT
    Vision Res; 1995 Nov; 35(22):3093-102. PubMed ID: 8533345
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Displacement of location in illusory line motion.
    Hubbard TL; Ruppel SE
    Psychol Res; 2013 May; 77(3):260-76. PubMed ID: 22398684
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Illusory temporal order for stimuli at different depth positions.
    Ichikawa M
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2009 Apr; 71(3):578-93. PubMed ID: 19304648
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Focal visual attention produces illusory temporal order and motion sensation.
    Hikosaka O; Miyauchi S; Shimojo S
    Vision Res; 1993 Jun; 33(9):1219-40. PubMed ID: 8333171
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Searching for illusory motion.
    Thornton IM; Zdravković S
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2020 Jan; 82(1):44-62. PubMed ID: 31119705
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Deciding what to see: the role of intention and attention in the perception of apparent motion.
    Kohler A; Haddad L; Singer W; Muckli L
    Vision Res; 2008 Mar; 48(8):1096-106. PubMed ID: 18279907
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Space, color, and direction of movement: how do they affect attention?
    Verghese A; Anderson AJ; Vidyasagar TR
    J Vis; 2013 Jul; 13(8):. PubMed ID: 23873673
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Visual attention and metacontrast modify latency to perception in opposite directions.
    Kirschfeld K; Kammer T
    Vision Res; 2000; 40(9):1027-33. PubMed ID: 10738061
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Visual motion sensation yielded by non-visually driven attention.
    Shimojo S; Miyauchi S; Hikosaka O
    Vision Res; 1997 Jun; 37(12):1575-80. PubMed ID: 9231224
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Illusory bending of a pursuit target.
    Debono K; Schütz AC; Gegenfurtner KR
    Vision Res; 2012 Mar; 57():51-60. PubMed ID: 22361410
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Revising the link between microsaccades and the spatial cueing of voluntary attention.
    Meyberg S; Sinn P; Engbert R; Sommer W
    Vision Res; 2017 Apr; 133():47-60. PubMed ID: 28163059
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.