These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

145 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 7738499)

  • 1. Overshadowing in landmark learning: touch-screen studies with pigeons and humans.
    Spetch ML
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 1995 Apr; 21(2):166-81. PubMed ID: 7738499
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Learning the configuration of a landmark array: I. Touch-screen studies with pigeons and humans.
    Spetch ML; Cheng K; MacDonald SE
    J Comp Psychol; 1996 Mar; 110(1):55-68. PubMed ID: 8851553
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Overshadowing between landmarks on the touchscreen and in arena with pigeons.
    Leising KJ; Garlick D; Blaisdell AP
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2011 Oct; 37(4):488-94. PubMed ID: 21604908
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Learning of absolute and relative distance and direction from discrete visual landmarks by pigeons (Columba livia).
    Sturz BR; Katz JS
    J Comp Psychol; 2009 Feb; 123(1):90-113. PubMed ID: 19236148
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Use of redundant sets of landmark information by humans (Homo sapiens) in a goal-searching task in an open field and on a computer screen.
    Sekiguchi K; Ushitani T; Sawa K
    J Comp Psychol; 2018 May; 132(2):178-188. PubMed ID: 29517247
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Flexible learning and use of multiple-landmark information by pigeons (Columba livia) in a touch screen-based goal searching task.
    Ushitani T; Jitsumori M
    J Comp Psychol; 2011 Aug; 125(3):317-27. PubMed ID: 21574692
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Spatial integration during performance in pigeons.
    Blaisdell AP; Schroeder JE; Fast CD
    Behav Processes; 2018 Sep; 154():73-80. PubMed ID: 29274761
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Landmark stability: further studies pointing to a role in spatial learning.
    Biegler R; Morris RG
    Q J Exp Psychol B; 1996 Nov; 49(4):307-45. PubMed ID: 8962538
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Learned predictiveness training modulates biases towards using boundary or landmark cues during navigation.
    Buckley MG; Smith AD; Haselgrove M
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2015; 68(6):1183-202. PubMed ID: 25409751
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Cue integration in spatial search for jointly learned landmarks but not for separately learned landmarks.
    Du Y; McMillan N; Madan CR; Spetch ML; Mou W
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2017 Dec; 43(12):1857-1871. PubMed ID: 28504533
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Absence of overshadowing between a landmark and geometric cues in a distinctively shaped environment: a test of Miller and Shettleworth (2007).
    McGregor A; Horne MR; Esber GR; Pearce JM
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2009 Jul; 35(3):357-70. PubMed ID: 19594281
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Clark's nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana) and the effects of goal--landmark distance on overshadowing.
    Goodyear AJ; Kamil AC
    J Comp Psychol; 2004 Sep; 118(3):258-64. PubMed ID: 15482053
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. No evidence for overshadowing or facilitation of spatial pattern learning by visual cues.
    Brown MF; Yang SY; DiGian KA
    Anim Learn Behav; 2002 Nov; 30(4):363-75. PubMed ID: 12593328
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Sensory preconditioning in spatial learning using a touch screen task in pigeons.
    Sawa K; Leising KJ; Blaisdell AP
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2005 Jul; 31(3):368-75. PubMed ID: 16045391
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Subjective landmarks in perception and memory for spatial location.
    Bryant DJ; Subbiah I
    Can J Exp Psychol; 1994 Mar; 48(1):119-39. PubMed ID: 8038766
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Stimulus control in the use of landmarks by pigeons in a touch-screen task.
    Cheng K; Spetch ML
    J Exp Anal Behav; 1995 Mar; 63(2):187-201. PubMed ID: 16812756
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Pigeons' (Columba livia) hierarchical organization of local and global cues in touch screen tasks.
    Legge EL; Spetch ML; Batty ER
    Behav Processes; 2009 Feb; 80(2):128-39. PubMed ID: 19022355
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. The effects of goal-landmark distance on overshadowing: A replication in humans (Homo sapiens) of Goodyear and Kamil (2004).
    Herrera E; Austen JM; Urcelay GP
    J Comp Psychol; 2024 May; 138(2):108-117. PubMed ID: 37732971
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Visual landmark orientation by flying bats at a large-scale touch and walk screen for bats, birds and rodents.
    Winter Y; von Merten S; Kleindienst HU
    J Neurosci Methods; 2005 Feb; 141(2):283-90. PubMed ID: 15661311
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Blocking of spatial learning between enclosure geometry and a local landmark.
    Wilson PN; Alexander T
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2008 Nov; 34(6):1369-76. PubMed ID: 18980401
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.