These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
124 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 7941388)
1. Integration of shading and texture cues: testing the linear model. Curran W; Johnston A Vision Res; 1994 Jul; 34(14):1863-74. PubMed ID: 7941388 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
2. The effect of illuminant position on perceived curvature. Curran W; Johnston A Vision Res; 1996 May; 36(10):1399-410. PubMed ID: 8762759 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
3. A perturbation analysis of depth perception from combinations of texture and motion cues. Young MJ; Landy MS; Maloney LT Vision Res; 1993 Dec; 33(18):2685-96. PubMed ID: 8296465 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
5. Texture and haptic cues in slant discrimination: reliability-based cue weighting without statistically optimal cue combination. Rosas P; Wagemans J; Ernst MO; Wichmann FA J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis; 2005 May; 22(5):801-9. PubMed ID: 15898539 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
6. Perceptual biases and cue weighting in perception of 3D slant from texture and stereo information. Saunders JA; Chen Z J Vis; 2015 Feb; 15(2):. PubMed ID: 25761332 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
7. First-order structure induces the 3-D curvature contrast effect. te Pas SF; Kappers AM Vision Res; 2001 Dec; 41(28):3829-35. PubMed ID: 11738450 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
8. Integration of stereopsis and motion shape cues. Johnston EB; Cumming BG; Landy MS Vision Res; 1994 Sep; 34(17):2259-75. PubMed ID: 7941420 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
9. Texture and object motion in slant discrimination: failure of reliability-based weighting of cues may be evidence for strong fusion. Rosas P; Wichmann FA; Wagemans J J Vis; 2007 Apr; 7(6):3. PubMed ID: 17685786 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
10. Perception of three-dimensional shape from texture is based on patterns of oriented energy. Li A; Zaidi Q Vision Res; 2000; 40(2):217-42. PubMed ID: 10793898 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
11. Optimal integration of shading and binocular disparity for depth perception. Lovell PG; Bloj M; Harris JM J Vis; 2012 Jan; 12(1):. PubMed ID: 22214563 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
12. Optimal integration of texture and motion cues to depth. Jacobs RA Vision Res; 1999 Oct; 39(21):3621-9. PubMed ID: 10746132 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
13. Disparity and shading cues cooperate for surface interpolation. Vuong QC; Domini F; Caudek C Perception; 2006; 35(2):145-55. PubMed ID: 16583761 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
14. Do we perceive a flattened world on the monitor screen? Domini F; Shah R; Caudek C Acta Psychol (Amst); 2011 Nov; 138(3):359-66. PubMed ID: 21986481 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
16. Depth from shading and disparity in humans and monkeys. Zhang Y; Weiner VS; Slocum WM; Schiller PH Vis Neurosci; 2007; 24(2):207-15. PubMed ID: 17640412 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
17. Do humans optimally integrate stereo and texture information for judgments of surface slant? Knill DC; Saunders JA Vision Res; 2003 Nov; 43(24):2539-58. PubMed ID: 13129541 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
18. Integration of depth modules: stereopsis and texture. Johnston EB; Cumming BG; Parker AJ Vision Res; 1993; 33(5-6):813-26. PubMed ID: 8351852 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
19. When texture takes precedence over motion in depth perception. O'Brien J; Johnston A Perception; 2000; 29(4):437-52. PubMed ID: 10953763 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]