These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

151 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 7975294)

  • 21. Motion detection on flashed, stationary pedestal gratings: evidence for an opponent-motion mechanism.
    Zemany L; Stromeyer CF; Chaparro A; Kronauer RE
    Vision Res; 1998 Mar; 38(6):795-812. PubMed ID: 9624431
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Pooling and segmenting motion signals.
    Burr DC; Baldassi S; Morrone MC; Verghese P
    Vision Res; 2009 Jun; 49(10):1065-72. PubMed ID: 19027034
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Thresholds for the identification of the direction of motion of plaid patterns defined by luminance or chromatic contrast.
    Gegenfurtner KR
    Vision Res; 1998 Mar; 38(6):881-8. PubMed ID: 9624437
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Hemifield relative motion bias in adults monocularly enucleated at an early age.
    Bowns L; Kirshner L; Steinbach M
    Vision Res; 1994 Dec; 34(24):3389-95. PubMed ID: 7863621
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Motion of chromatic stimuli: first-order or second-order?
    Cropper SJ; Derrington AM
    Vision Res; 1994 Jan; 34(1):49-58. PubMed ID: 8116268
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Position displacement, not velocity, is the cue to motion detection of second-order stimuli.
    Seiffert AE; Cavanagh P
    Vision Res; 1998 Nov; 38(22):3569-82. PubMed ID: 9893790
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Velocity discrimination at constant multiples of threshold contrast.
    Panish SC
    Vision Res; 1988; 28(2):193-201. PubMed ID: 3414005
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Psychophysical evidence for a radial motion bias in complex motion discrimination.
    Beardsley SA; Vaina LM
    Vision Res; 2005 Jun; 45(12):1569-86. PubMed ID: 15781074
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Lower thresholds of motion for gratings as a function of eccentricity and contrast.
    Johnston A; Wright MJ
    Vision Res; 1985; 25(2):179-85. PubMed ID: 4013086
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Temporal impulse response functions for luminance and colour during saccades.
    Burr DC; Morrone MC
    Vision Res; 1996 Jul; 36(14):2069-78. PubMed ID: 8776473
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Discrimination of moving gratings at and above detection threshold.
    Thompson P
    Vision Res; 1983; 23(12):1533-8. PubMed ID: 6666055
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Coherence thresholds for discrimination of motion direction in infants.
    Wattam-Bell J
    Vision Res; 1994 Apr; 34(7):877-83. PubMed ID: 8160401
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Optical blur and the perception of global coherent motion in random dot cinematograms.
    Barton JJ; Rizzo M; Nawrot M; Simpson T
    Vision Res; 1996 Oct; 36(19):3051-9. PubMed ID: 8917768
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Spatial-frequency discrimination of drifting gratings.
    Greenlee MW; Gerling J; Waltenspiel S
    Vision Res; 1990; 30(9):1331-9. PubMed ID: 2219749
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Perceived speed of moving two-dimensional patterns.
    Ferrera VP; Wilson HR
    Vision Res; 1991; 31(5):877-93. PubMed ID: 2035271
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Effects of spatial frequency, duration, and contrast on discriminating motion directions.
    Yang J; Stevenson SB
    J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis; 1997 Sep; 14(9):2041-8. PubMed ID: 9291600
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. The optimal displacement for the detection of motion.
    Boulton JC; Hess RF
    Vision Res; 1990; 30(7):1101-6. PubMed ID: 2392838
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. The aperture problem--I. Perception of nonrigidity and motion direction in translating sinusoidal lines.
    Nakayama K; Silverman GH
    Vision Res; 1988; 28(6):739-46. PubMed ID: 3227650
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. The analysis of motion of two-dimensional patterns: do Fourier components provide the first stage?
    Cox MJ; Derrington AM
    Vision Res; 1994 Jan; 34(1):59-72. PubMed ID: 8116269
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Perceived direction of moving two-dimensional patterns.
    Ferrera VP; Wilson HR
    Vision Res; 1990; 30(2):273-87. PubMed ID: 2309462
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.