These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

270 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 8015605)

  • 1. A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children.
    Hermer L; Spelke ES
    Nature; 1994 Jul; 370(6484):57-9. PubMed ID: 8015605
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Reorientation by geometric and landmark information in environments of different size.
    Vallortigara G; Feruglio M; Sovrano VA
    Dev Sci; 2005 Sep; 8(5):393-401. PubMed ID: 16048511
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Separate geometric and non-geometric modules for spatial reorientation: evidence from a lopsided animal brain.
    Vallortigara G; Pagni P; Sovrano VA
    J Cogn Neurosci; 2004 Apr; 16(3):390-400. PubMed ID: 15072675
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Why size counts: children's spatial reorientation in large and small enclosures.
    Learmonth AE; Newcombe NS; Sheridan N; Jones M
    Dev Sci; 2008 May; 11(3):414-26. PubMed ID: 18466375
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Experience and geometry: controlled-rearing studies with chicks.
    Chiandetti C; Vallortigara G
    Anim Cogn; 2010 May; 13(3):463-70. PubMed ID: 19960217
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Young children's use of features to reorient is more than just associative: further evidence against a modular view of spatial processing.
    Newcombe NS; Ratliff KR; Shallcross WL; Twyman AD
    Dev Sci; 2010 Jan; 13(1):213-20. PubMed ID: 20121877
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Toddlers' use of metric information and landmarks to reorient.
    Learmonth AE; Newcombe NS; Huttenlocher J
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2001 Nov; 80(3):225-44. PubMed ID: 11583524
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Modularity and spatial reorientation in a simple mind: encoding of geometric and nongeometric properties of a spatial environment by fish.
    Sovrano VA; Bisazza A; Vallortigara G
    Cognition; 2002 Sep; 85(2):B51-9. PubMed ID: 12127704
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Location representation in enclosed spaces: what types of information afford young children an advantage?
    Lourenco SF; Addy D; Huttenlocher J
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2009 Nov; 104(3):313-25. PubMed ID: 19560782
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Is there an innate geometric module? Effects of experience with angular geometric cues on spatial re-orientation based on the shape of the environment.
    Chiandetti C; Vallortigara G
    Anim Cogn; 2008 Jan; 11(1):139-46. PubMed ID: 17629754
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Spatial encoding in mountain chickadees: features overshadow geometry.
    Gray ER; Bloomfield LL; Ferrey A; Spetch ML; Sturdy CB
    Biol Lett; 2005 Sep; 1(3):314-7. PubMed ID: 17148196
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Spatial reorientation in rats (Rattus norvegicus): use of geometric and featural information as a function of arena size and feature location.
    Maes JH; Fontanari L; Regolin L
    Behav Brain Res; 2009 Aug; 201(2):285-91. PubMed ID: 19428646
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Doing Socrates experiment right: controlled rearing studies of geometrical knowledge in animals.
    Vallortigara G; Sovrano VA; Chiandetti C
    Curr Opin Neurobiol; 2009 Feb; 19(1):20-6. PubMed ID: 19299120
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Is language necessary for human spatial reorientation? Reconsidering evidence from dual task paradigms.
    Ratliff KR; Newcombe NS
    Cogn Psychol; 2008 Mar; 56(2):142-63. PubMed ID: 17663986
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Lateralized fish perform better than nonlateralized fish in spatial reorientation tasks.
    Sovrano VA; Dadda M; Bisazza A
    Behav Brain Res; 2005 Aug; 163(1):122-7. PubMed ID: 15941602
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Spatial reorientation: the effects of space size on the encoding of landmark and geometry information.
    Chiandetti C; Regolin L; Sovrano VA; Vallortigara G
    Anim Cogn; 2007 Apr; 10(2):159-68. PubMed ID: 17136416
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Rats' use of geometric, featural and orientation cues to locate a hidden goal.
    Batty ER; Hoban L; Spetch ML; Dickson CT
    Behav Processes; 2009 Nov; 82(3):327-34. PubMed ID: 19683037
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Animals' use of landmarks and metric information to reorient: effects of the size of the experimental space.
    Sovrano VA; Bisazza A; Vallortigara G
    Cognition; 2005 Sep; 97(2):121-33. PubMed ID: 16226559
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Sources of flexibility in human cognition: dual-task studies of space and language.
    Hermer-Vazquez L; Spelke ES; Katsnelson AS
    Cogn Psychol; 1999 Aug; 39(1):3-36. PubMed ID: 10433786
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Reorienting when cues conflict: evidence for an adaptive-combination view.
    Ratliff KR; Newcombe NS
    Psychol Sci; 2008 Dec; 19(12):1301-7. PubMed ID: 19121141
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 14.