These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

168 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 8236859)

  • 1. Shape from texture: ideal observers and human psychophysics.
    Blake A; Bülthoff HH; Sheinberg D
    Vision Res; 1993 Aug; 33(12):1723-37. PubMed ID: 8236859
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Ideal observer perturbation analysis reveals human strategies for inferring surface orientation from texture.
    Knill DC
    Vision Res; 1998 Sep; 38(17):2635-56. PubMed ID: 12116709
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Surface orientation from texture: ideal observers, generic observers and the information content of texture cues.
    Knill DC
    Vision Res; 1998 Jun; 38(11):1655-82. PubMed ID: 9747502
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Robust cue integration: a Bayesian model and evidence from cue-conflict studies with stereoscopic and figure cues to slant.
    Knill DC
    J Vis; 2007 May; 7(7):5.1-24. PubMed ID: 17685801
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Does the human visual system implement an ideal observer theory of slant from texture?
    Buckley D; Frisby JP; Blake A
    Vision Res; 1996 Apr; 36(8):1163-76. PubMed ID: 8762720
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Erratum to "Information limitations in perception of shape from texture". [Vision Research 41 (2001) 1519-1534].
    Li A; Zaidi Q
    Vision Res; 2001 Oct; 41(22):2927-42. PubMed ID: 11701185
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Information limitations in perception of shape from texture.
    Li A; Zaidi Q
    Vision Res; 2001 May; 41(12):1519-33. PubMed ID: 11343719
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. The perception of 3D shape from texture based on directional width gradients.
    Todd JT; Thaler L
    J Vis; 2010 May; 10(5):17. PubMed ID: 20616135
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. The perception of 3D shape from planar cut contours.
    Egan EJ; Todd JT; Phillips F
    J Vis; 2011 Oct; 11(12):1-13. PubMed ID: 22016555
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. The effects of phase on the perception of 3D shape from texture: psychophysics and modeling.
    Thaler L; Todd JT; Dijkstra TM
    Vision Res; 2007 Feb; 47(3):411-27. PubMed ID: 17141292
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Characterization of the spatial-frequency spectrum in the perception of shape from texture.
    Sakai K; Finkel LH
    J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis; 1995 Jun; 12(6):1208-24. PubMed ID: 7769507
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Do humans optimally integrate stereo and texture information for judgments of surface slant?
    Knill DC; Saunders JA
    Vision Res; 2003 Nov; 43(24):2539-58. PubMed ID: 13129541
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Surface orientation from texture: isotropy or homogeneity (or both)?
    Rosenholtz R; Malik J
    Vision Res; 1997 Aug; 37(16):2283-93. PubMed ID: 9578909
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Effects of foreground scale in texture discrimination tasks: performance is size, shape, and content specific.
    Rubenstein BS; Sagi D
    Spat Vis; 1993; 7(4):293-310. PubMed ID: 8110630
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Texture and object motion in slant discrimination: failure of reliability-based weighting of cues may be evidence for strong fusion.
    Rosas P; Wichmann FA; Wagemans J
    J Vis; 2007 Apr; 7(6):3. PubMed ID: 17685786
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Experience-dependent integration of texture and motion cues to depth.
    Jacobs RA; Fine I
    Vision Res; 1999 Dec; 39(24):4062-75. PubMed ID: 10748939
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. "Phase capture" in the perception of interpolated shape: cue combination and the influence function.
    Levi DM; Wing-Hong Li R; Klein SA
    Vision Res; 2003 Sep; 43(21):2233-43. PubMed ID: 12885377
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Influence of surface attitude and curvature scaling on discrimination of binocularly presented curved surfaces.
    de Vries SC; Kappers AM; Koenderink JJ
    Vision Res; 1994 Sep; 34(18):2409-23. PubMed ID: 7975280
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Oriented texture detection: ideal observer modelling and classification image analysis.
    Or CC; Elder JH
    J Vis; 2011 Jul; 11(8):1-20. PubMed ID: 21795411
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. An ideal observer with channels versus feature-independent processing of spatial frequency and orientation in visual search performance.
    Shimozaki SS; Eckstein MP; Abbey CK
    J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis; 2003 Dec; 20(12):2197-215. PubMed ID: 14686499
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 9.