These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

273 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 8245918)

  • 1. Detecting slant-in-depth of real trapezoidal and rectangular surfaces: moving-monocular viewing equivalent to stationary-binocular viewing.
    Reinhardt-Rutland AH
    J Gen Psychol; 1993 Apr; 120(2):177-85. PubMed ID: 8245918
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Perceiving the orientation-in-depth of triangular surfaces: static-monocular, moving-monocular, and static-binocular viewing.
    Reinhardt-Rutland AH
    J Gen Psychol; 1996 Jan; 123(1):19-28. PubMed ID: 8901208
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Primary depth cues and background pattern in the portrayal of slant.
    Reinhardt-Rutland AH
    J Gen Psychol; 1992 Jan; 119(1):29-35. PubMed ID: 1613487
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Detecting orientation of a surface: the rectangularity postulate and primary depth cues.
    Reinhardt-Rutland AH
    J Gen Psychol; 1990 Oct; 117(4):391-401. PubMed ID: 2286836
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Perceiving the orientation in depth of real surfaces: background pattern affects motion and pictorial information.
    Reinhardt-Rutland AH
    Perception; 1995; 24(4):405-14. PubMed ID: 7675620
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. The framing effect with rectangular and trapezoidal surfaces: actual and pictorial surface slant, frame orientation, and viewing condition.
    Reinhardt-Rutland AH
    Perception; 1999; 28(11):1361-71. PubMed ID: 10755145
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Perceiving surface orientation: pictorial information based on rectangularity can be overriden during observer motion.
    Reinhardt-Rutland AH
    Perception; 1993; 22(3):335-41. PubMed ID: 8316520
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Parallactic movement beats binocularity in the presence of external visual noise.
    Voges N; Bach M; Kommerell G
    Ophthalmic Physiol Opt; 2012 Jul; 32(4):308-16. PubMed ID: 22697215
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. The processing of linear perspective and binocular information for action and perception.
    Bruggeman H; Yonas A; Konczak J
    Neuropsychologia; 2007 Apr; 45(7):1420-6. PubMed ID: 17166526
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. The Influence of Monocular Spatial Cues on Vergence Eye Movements in Monocular and Binocular Viewing of 3-D and 2-D Stimuli.
    Batvinionak AA; Gracheva MA; Bolshakov AS; Rozhkova GI
    Perception; 2015; 44(8-9):1077-84. PubMed ID: 26562921
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Depth judgments of triangular surfaces during moving monocular viewing.
    Reinhardt-Rutland AH
    Perception; 1996; 25(1):27-35. PubMed ID: 8861168
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. The role of perceptual cues in the trapezoid oscillatory illusion.
    Martinetti RF
    J Gen Psychol; 1975 Jan; 92(1st Half):129-35. PubMed ID: 1113085
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Virtual slant explains perceived slant, distortion, and motion in pictorial scenes.
    Erkelens CJ
    Perception; 2013; 42(3):253-70. PubMed ID: 23837203
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Apparent motion of monocular stimuli in different depth planes with lateral head movements.
    Shimono K; Tam WJ; Ono H
    Vision Res; 2007 Apr; 47(8):1027-35. PubMed ID: 17337029
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. The role of stereopsis, motion parallax, perspective and angle polarity in perceiving 3-D shape.
    Sherman A; Papathomas TV; Jain A; Keane BP
    Seeing Perceiving; 2012; 25(3-4):263-85. PubMed ID: 21771395
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Distance impairs postural stability only under binocular viewing.
    LĂȘ TT; Kapoula Z
    Vision Res; 2006 Oct; 46(21):3586-93. PubMed ID: 16899270
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Pictorial cues constrain depth in da Vinci stereopsis.
    Makino Y; Yano M
    Vision Res; 2006 Jan; 46(1-2):91-105. PubMed ID: 16271743
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Motion parallax enables depth processing for action in a visual form agnosic when binocular vision is unavailable.
    Dijkerman HC; Milner AD; Carey DP
    Neuropsychologia; 1999 Dec; 37(13):1505-10. PubMed ID: 10617271
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Quantitative perceived depth from sequential monocular decamouflage.
    Brooks KR; Gillam BJ
    Vision Res; 2006 Mar; 46(5):605-13. PubMed ID: 16054190
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Perceived rigidity in motion-in-depth increases with contour perspective.
    Vienne C; Blondé L; Mamassian P
    Perception; 2014; 43(6):481-98. PubMed ID: 25154283
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 14.