These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

161 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 8278665)

  • 1. Children's self-regulated learning of movement sequences.
    Bouffard M; Dunn JG
    Res Q Exerc Sport; 1993 Dec; 64(4):393-403. PubMed ID: 8278665
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Effects of strategy use on children's motor performance in a continuous timing task.
    Liu T; Jensen JL
    Res Q Exerc Sport; 2011 Jun; 82(2):198-209. PubMed ID: 21699099
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Acquisition of Motor and Cognitive Skills through Repetition in Typically Developing Children.
    Magallón S; Narbona J; Crespo-Eguílaz N
    PLoS One; 2016; 11(7):e0158684. PubMed ID: 27384671
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Sequential pointing in children and adults.
    Badan M; Hauert CA; Mounoud P
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2000 Jan; 75(1):43-69. PubMed ID: 10660903
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Developmental and individual differences in young children's use and maintenance of a selective memory strategy.
    Schwenck C; Bjorklund DF; Schneider W
    Dev Psychol; 2009 Jul; 45(4):1034-50. PubMed ID: 19586178
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Limits on movement integration in children: The concatenation of trained subsequences into composite sequences as a specific experience-triggered skill.
    Ashtamker L; Karni A
    Neurobiol Learn Mem; 2015 Sep; 123():58-66. PubMed ID: 26004677
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Developmental changes and the effect of self-generated feedback in metacognitive controlled spacing strategies in 7-year-olds, 10-year-olds, and adults.
    Tsalas N; Paulus M; Sodian B
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2015 Apr; 132():140-54. PubMed ID: 25703006
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Temporal organization in children's strategy formation.
    Hock HS; Park CL; Bjorklund DF
    J Exp Child Psychol; 1998 Sep; 70(3):187-206. PubMed ID: 9742179
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Central executive involvement in children's spatial memory.
    Ang SY; Lee K
    Memory; 2008 Nov; 16(8):918-33. PubMed ID: 18802804
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Reducing errors benefits the field-based learning of a fundamental movement skill in children.
    Capio CM; Poolton JM; Sit CH; Holmstrom M; Masters RS
    Scand J Med Sci Sports; 2013 Mar; 23(2):181-8. PubMed ID: 22092534
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Children's questions: a mechanism for cognitive development.
    Chouinard MM
    Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 2007; 72(1):vii-ix, 1-112; discussion 113-26. PubMed ID: 17394580
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Sleep-dependent consolidation of procedural motor memories in children and adults: the pre-sleep level of performance matters.
    Wilhelm I; Metzkow-Mészàros M; Knapp S; Born J
    Dev Sci; 2012 Jul; 15(4):506-15. PubMed ID: 22709400
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. A comparison between children's and adults' ability to detect conceptual information conveyed through representational gestures.
    Kelly SD; Church RB
    Child Dev; 1998 Feb; 69(1):85-93. PubMed ID: 9499559
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Recalling demonstrated and guided movements using imaginary and verbal rehearsal strategies.
    Hall C; Moore J; Annett J; Rodgers W
    Res Q Exerc Sport; 1997 Jun; 68(2):136-44. PubMed ID: 9200247
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Sequence memory skills in Spastic Bilateral Cerebral Palsy are age independent as in normally developing children.
    Gagliardi C; Tavano A; Turconi AC; Borgatti R
    Disabil Rehabil; 2013 Mar; 35(6):506-12. PubMed ID: 22897566
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. The performance of infants born preterm and full-term in the mobile paradigm: learning and memory.
    Heathcock JC; Bhat AN; Lobo MA; Galloway JC
    Phys Ther; 2004 Sep; 84(9):808-21. PubMed ID: 15330694
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Across the great divide: bridging the gap between understanding of toddlers' and older children's thinking.
    Chen Z; Siegler RS
    Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 2000; 65(2):i-vii, 1-96. PubMed ID: 12467094
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Movement substructures change as a function of practice in children and adults.
    Thomas JR; Yan JH; Stelmach GE
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2000 Mar; 75(3):228-44. PubMed ID: 10666326
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Three-year-old children can access their own memory to guide responses on a visual matching task.
    Balcomb FK; Gerken L
    Dev Sci; 2008 Sep; 11(5):750-60. PubMed ID: 18801131
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Nap-dependent learning in infants.
    Hupbach A; Gomez RL; Bootzin RR; Nadel L
    Dev Sci; 2009 Nov; 12(6):1007-12. PubMed ID: 19840054
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 9.