These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

102 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 8432092)

  • 1. Reconstructive remembering of the scientific literature.
    Vicente KJ; Brewer WF
    Cognition; 1993 Feb; 46(2):101-28. PubMed ID: 8432092
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Expertise in complex decision making: the role of search in chess 70 years after de Groot.
    Connors MH; Burns BD; Campitelli G
    Cogn Sci; 2011; 35(8):1567-79. PubMed ID: 21981829
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Recall or evaluation of chess positions revisited: the relationship between memory and evaluation in chess skill.
    Schultetus RS; Charness N
    Am J Psychol; 1999; 112(4):555-69. PubMed ID: 10696266
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. The role of constraints in expert memory.
    Gobet F; Waters AJ
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2003 Nov; 29(6):1082-94. PubMed ID: 14622048
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. The meaning of "significance" for different types of research [translated and annotated by Eric-Jan Wagenmakers, Denny Borsboom, Josine Verhagen, Rogier Kievit, Marjan Bakker, Angelique Cramer, Dora Matzke, Don Mellenbergh, and Han L. J. van der Maas]. 1969.
    de Groot AD
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2014 May; 148():188-94. PubMed ID: 24589374
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Reading and visual memory: remembering scenes that were never seen.
    Intraub H; Hoffman JE
    Am J Psychol; 1992; 105(1):101-14. PubMed ID: 1605323
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Recall of random and distorted chess positions: implications for the theory of expertise.
    Gobet F; Simon HA
    Mem Cognit; 1996 Jul; 24(4):493-503. PubMed ID: 8757497
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Chess knowledge predicts chess memory even after controlling for chess experience: Evidence for the role of high-level processes.
    Lane DM; Chang YA
    Mem Cognit; 2018 Apr; 46(3):337-348. PubMed ID: 29101550
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Working memory in chess.
    Robbins TW; Anderson EJ; Barker DR; Bradley AC; Fearnyhough C; Henson R; Hudson SR
    Mem Cognit; 1996 Jan; 24(1):83-93. PubMed ID: 8822160
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Counting on working memory in arithmetic problem solving.
    Logie RH; Gilhooly KJ; Wynn V
    Mem Cognit; 1994 Jul; 22(4):395-410. PubMed ID: 7934946
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Recall of briefly presented chess positions and its relation to chess skill.
    Gong Y; Ericsson KA; Moxley JH
    PLoS One; 2015; 10(3):e0118756. PubMed ID: 25774693
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Problem-oriented training promotes spontaneous analogical transfer: memory-oriented training promotes memory for training.
    Needham DR; Begg IM
    Mem Cognit; 1991 Nov; 19(6):543-57. PubMed ID: 1758301
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Metamemories of memory researchers.
    Park DC; Smith AD; Cavanaugh JC
    Mem Cognit; 1990 May; 18(3):321-7. PubMed ID: 2355861
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Reconstructive memory in the dating of personal and public news events.
    Larsen SF; Thompson CP
    Mem Cognit; 1995 Nov; 23(6):780-90. PubMed ID: 8538449
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. An asymmetry between memory encoding and retrieval. Revelation, generation, and transfer-appropriate processing.
    Mulligan NW; Lozito JP
    Psychol Sci; 2006 Jan; 17(1):7-11. PubMed ID: 16371137
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Working memory: activation limitations on retrieval.
    Anderson JR; Reder LM; Lebiere C
    Cogn Psychol; 1996 Jun; 30(3):221-56. PubMed ID: 8660785
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Reinstating study context produces unconscious influences of memory.
    Allen SW; Jacoby LL
    Mem Cognit; 1990 May; 18(3):270-8. PubMed ID: 2355856
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. An examination of the continuous distractor task and the "long-term recency effect".
    Koppenaal L; Glanzer M
    Mem Cognit; 1990 Mar; 18(2):183-95. PubMed ID: 2319960
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Conjunction errors, recollection-based rejections, and forgetting in a continuous recognition task.
    Jones TC; Atchley P
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2006 Jan; 32(1):70-8. PubMed ID: 16478341
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Memorabeatlia: a naturalistic study of long-term memory.
    Hyman IE; Rubin DC
    Mem Cognit; 1990 Mar; 18(2):205-14. PubMed ID: 2319962
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 6.