These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

89 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 8750081)

  • 1. A phase plane representation of rat exploratory behavior.
    Tchernichovski O; Golani I
    J Neurosci Methods; 1995 Nov; 62(1-2):21-7. PubMed ID: 8750081
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. The dynamics of long-term exploration in the rat. Part I. A phase-plane analysis of the relationship between location and velocity.
    Tchernichovski O; Benjamini Y; Golani I
    Biol Cybern; 1998 Jun; 78(6):423-32. PubMed ID: 9711816
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. The dynamics of long-term exploration in the rat. Part II. An analytical model of the kinematic structure of rat exploratory behavior.
    Tchernichovski O; Benjamini Y
    Biol Cybern; 1998 Jun; 78(6):433-40. PubMed ID: 9711817
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Quantification of a single exploratory trip reveals hippocampal formation mediated dead reckoning.
    Wallace DG; Hines DJ; Whishaw IQ
    J Neurosci Methods; 2002 Jan; 113(2):131-45. PubMed ID: 11772435
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. The point of entry contributes to the organization of exploratory behavior of rats on an open field: an example of spontaneous episodic memory.
    Nemati F; Whishaw IQ
    Behav Brain Res; 2007 Aug; 182(1):119-28. PubMed ID: 17590451
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Exploration in a dark open field: a shift from directional to positional progression and a proposed model of acquiring spatial information.
    Avni R; Zadicario P; Eilam D
    Behav Brain Res; 2006 Aug; 171(2):313-23. PubMed ID: 16712970
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Fimbria-fornix lesions disrupt the dead reckoning (homing) component of exploratory behavior in mice.
    Gorny JH; Gorny B; Wallace DG; Whishaw IQ
    Learn Mem; 2002; 9(6):387-94. PubMed ID: 12464698
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Medial septum lesions disrupt exploratory trip organization: evidence for septohippocampal involvement in dead reckoning.
    Martin MM; Horn KL; Kusman KJ; Wallace DG
    Physiol Behav; 2007 Feb; 90(2-3):412-24. PubMed ID: 17126862
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Home base behavior of rats (Rattus norvegicus) exploring a novel environment.
    Eilam D; Golani I
    Behav Brain Res; 1989 Sep; 34(3):199-211. PubMed ID: 2789700
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Detailed analysis of the behavior of Lister and Wistar rats in anxiety, object recognition and object location tasks.
    Ennaceur A; Michalikova S; Bradford A; Ahmed S
    Behav Brain Res; 2005 Apr; 159(2):247-66. PubMed ID: 15817188
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Comparison of the effects of entorhinal and retrosplenial cortical lesions on habituation, reaction to spatial and non-spatial changes during object exploration in the rat.
    Parron C; Save E
    Neurobiol Learn Mem; 2004 Jul; 82(1):1-11. PubMed ID: 15183166
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Calibrating space: exploration is important for allothetic and idiothetic navigation.
    Whishaw IQ; Brooks BL
    Hippocampus; 1999; 9(6):659-67. PubMed ID: 10641759
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation.
    Cheng K
    Cognition; 1986 Jul; 23(2):149-78. PubMed ID: 3742991
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Home bases formed to visual cues but not to self-movement (dead reckoning) cues in exploring hippocampectomized rats.
    Hines DJ; Whishaw IQ
    Eur J Neurosci; 2005 Nov; 22(9):2363-75. PubMed ID: 16262675
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Exploration and navigation in the blind mole rat (Spalax ehrenbergi): global calibration as a primer of spatial representation.
    Avni R; Tzvaigrach Y; Eilam D
    J Exp Biol; 2008 Sep; 211(Pt 17):2817-26. PubMed ID: 18723540
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. 'Looping'-an exploration mechanism in a dark open field.
    Zadicario P; Avni R; Zadicario E; Eilam D
    Behav Brain Res; 2005 Apr; 159(1):27-36. PubMed ID: 15794994
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. A rat model of psychopathology for use in exercise science.
    Morgan WP; Olson EB; Pedersen NP
    Med Sci Sports Exerc; 1982; 14(1):91-100. PubMed ID: 7070264
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Neonatal hippocampal damage in rats: long-term spatial memory deficits and associations with magnitude of hippocampal damage.
    Altemus KL; Almli CR
    Hippocampus; 1997; 7(4):403-15. PubMed ID: 9287080
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Attentional processes and learning and memory in rats: the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus compared.
    Broersen LM
    Prog Brain Res; 2000; 126():79-94. PubMed ID: 11105641
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. SEE: a tool for the visualization and analysis of rodent exploratory behavior.
    Drai D; Golani I
    Neurosci Biobehav Rev; 2001 Jul; 25(5):409-26. PubMed ID: 11566479
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 5.