These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
3. Coherence thresholds for discrimination of motion direction in infants. Wattam-Bell J Vision Res; 1994 Apr; 34(7):877-83. PubMed ID: 8160401 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
4. Human velocity and direction discrimination measured with random dot patterns. De Bruyn B; Orban GA Vision Res; 1988; 28(12):1323-35. PubMed ID: 3256150 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
5. The development of maximum displacement limits for discrimination of motion direction in infancy. Wattam-Bell J Vision Res; 1992 Apr; 32(4):621-30. PubMed ID: 1413546 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
6. Perception of opposite-moving dots in 3- to 5-month-old infants. Kanazawa S; Shirai N; Ohtsuka Y; Yamaguchi MK Vision Res; 2006 Feb; 46(3):346-56. PubMed ID: 16183098 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
16. The effects of spatiotemporal integration on maximum displacement thresholds for the detection of coherent motion. Todd JT; Norman JF Vision Res; 1995 Aug; 35(16):2287-302. PubMed ID: 7571465 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
18. The analysis of motion of two-dimensional patterns: do Fourier components provide the first stage? Cox MJ; Derrington AM Vision Res; 1994 Jan; 34(1):59-72. PubMed ID: 8116269 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
19. Velocity perception and discrimination: relation to temporal mechanisms. Smith AT Vision Res; 1987; 27(9):1491-500. PubMed ID: 3445483 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
20. Infant color vision: moving tritan stimuli do not elicit directionally appropriate eye movements in 2- and 4-month-olds. Teller DY; Brooks TE; Palmer J Vision Res; 1997 Apr; 37(7):899-911. PubMed ID: 9156187 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related] [Next] [New Search]