These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

126 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 8851557)

  • 1. Hiding and perspective taking in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis).
    Kummer H; Anzenberger G; Hemelrijk CK
    J Comp Psychol; 1996 Mar; 110(1):97-102. PubMed ID: 8851557
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Spontaneous use of matching visual cues during foraging by long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis).
    Menzel CR
    J Comp Psychol; 1996 Dec; 110(4):370-6. PubMed ID: 8956507
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Seeing the experimenter influences the response to pointing cues in long-tailed macaques.
    Schmitt V; Schloegl C; Fischer J
    PLoS One; 2014; 9(3):e91348. PubMed ID: 24646501
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Lack of evidence that Tonkean macaques understand what others can hear.
    Costes-Thiré M; Levé M; Uhlrich P; De Marco A; Thierry B
    Anim Cogn; 2015 Jan; 18(1):251-8. PubMed ID: 25120070
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Long-tailed macaques display unexpected waiting abilities in exchange tasks.
    Pelé M; Dufour V; Micheletta J; Thierry B
    Anim Cogn; 2010 Mar; 13(2):263-71. PubMed ID: 19597853
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Do rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) attribute knowledge and ignorance to others?
    Povinelli DJ; Parks KA; Novak MA
    J Comp Psychol; 1991 Dec; 105(4):318-25. PubMed ID: 1778064
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Natural choice in nonhuman primates.
    Silberberg A; Widholm JJ; Bresler D; Fujita K; Anderson JR
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 1998 Apr; 24(2):215-28. PubMed ID: 9556910
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. No costly prosociality among related long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis).
    Sterck EH; Olesen CU; Massen JJ
    J Comp Psychol; 2015 Aug; 129(3):275-82. PubMed ID: 25961651
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Effect of others' attentional states on vocalizations in Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata).
    Yamaguchi C; Izumi A
    Behav Processes; 2006 Nov; 73(3):285-9. PubMed ID: 16926072
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. The long-term retention of events in monkey memory.
    Gower EC
    Behav Brain Res; 1990 May; 38(3):191-8. PubMed ID: 2363839
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Understanding of and reasoning about object-object relationships in long-tailed macaques?
    Schloegl C; Waldmann MR; Fischer J
    Anim Cogn; 2013 May; 16(3):493-507. PubMed ID: 23417558
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Pictorial gaze cues do not enhance long-tailed macaques' performance on a computerised object-location task.
    Vick SJ; Toxopeus I; Anderson JR
    Behav Processes; 2006 Nov; 73(3):308-14. PubMed ID: 16962253
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Tool-use training temporarily enhances cognitive performance in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis).
    Tia B; Viaro R; Fadiga L
    Anim Cogn; 2018 May; 21(3):365-378. PubMed ID: 29502167
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. The role of anthropic, ecological, and social factors in sleeping site choice by long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis).
    Brotcorne F; Maslarov C; Wandia IN; Fuentes A; Beudels-Jamar RC; Huynen MC
    Am J Primatol; 2014 Dec; 76(12):1140-50. PubMed ID: 24810544
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) understand what conspecifics can see in a competitive situation.
    Overduin-de Vries AM; Spruijt BM; Sterck EH
    Anim Cogn; 2014 Jan; 17(1):77-84. PubMed ID: 23666397
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Benefiting friends or dominants: prosocial choices mainly depend on rank position in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis).
    Massen JJ; Luyten IJ; Spruijt BM; Sterck EH
    Primates; 2011 Jul; 52(3):237-47. PubMed ID: 21416218
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Time preferences in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) and humans (Homo sapiens).
    Genty E; Karpel H; Silberberg A
    Anim Cogn; 2012 Nov; 15(6):1161-72. PubMed ID: 22843198
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Stimulus-food pairings produce stimulus-directed touch-screen responding in cynomolgus monkeys (macaca fascicularis) with or without a positive response contingency.
    Bullock CE; Myers TM
    J Exp Anal Behav; 2009 Jul; 92(1):41-55. PubMed ID: 20119521
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Distinguishing intentional from accidental actions in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and human children (Homo sapiens).
    Call J; Tomasello M
    J Comp Psychol; 1998 Jun; 112(2):192-206. PubMed ID: 9642787
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Macaque-human interactions and the societal perceptions of macaques in Singapore.
    Sha JC; Gumert MD; Lee BP; Jones-Engel L; Chan S; Fuentes A
    Am J Primatol; 2009 Oct; 71(10):825-39. PubMed ID: 19479953
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.