These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

373 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 8962538)

  • 1. Landmark stability: further studies pointing to a role in spatial learning.
    Biegler R; Morris RG
    Q J Exp Psychol B; 1996 Nov; 49(4):307-45. PubMed ID: 8962538
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Landmark stability: studies exploring whether the perceived stability of the environment influences spatial representation.
    Biegler R; Morris R
    J Exp Biol; 1996 Jan; 199(Pt 1):187-93. PubMed ID: 9317604
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Learning of absolute and relative distance and direction from discrete visual landmarks by pigeons (Columba livia).
    Sturz BR; Katz JS
    J Comp Psychol; 2009 Feb; 123(1):90-113. PubMed ID: 19236148
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Olfactory cues potentiate learning of distant visuospatial information.
    Lavenex P; Schenk F
    Neurobiol Learn Mem; 1997 Sep; 68(2):140-53. PubMed ID: 9322257
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Flexible learning and use of multiple-landmark information by pigeons (Columba livia) in a touch screen-based goal searching task.
    Ushitani T; Jitsumori M
    J Comp Psychol; 2011 Aug; 125(3):317-27. PubMed ID: 21574692
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Use of a geometric rule or absolute vectors: landmark use by Clark's nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana).
    Kelly DM; Kippenbrock S; Templeton J; Kamil AC
    Brain Res Bull; 2008 Jun; 76(3):293-9. PubMed ID: 18498944
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Blocking in the spatial domain with arrays of discrete landmarks.
    Biegler R; Morris RG
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 1999 Jul; 25(3):334-51. PubMed ID: 10423856
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Cue reliability and a landmark stability heuristic determine relative weighting between egocentric and allocentric visual information in memory-guided reach.
    Byrne PA; Crawford JD
    J Neurophysiol; 2010 Jun; 103(6):3054-69. PubMed ID: 20457858
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Landmark stability is a prerequisite for spatial but not discrimination learning.
    Biegler R; Morris RG
    Nature; 1993 Feb; 361(6413):631-3. PubMed ID: 8437622
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Egocentric and allocentric search: effects of platform distance and environmental cues.
    Tamara C; Leffel J; Timberlake W
    Anim Cogn; 2010 May; 13(3):565-81. PubMed ID: 20063192
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Facilitation of learning spatial relations among locations by visual cues: generality across spatial configurations.
    Sturz BR; Kelly DM; Brown MF
    Anim Cogn; 2010 Mar; 13(2):341-9. PubMed ID: 19777275
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Rats (Rattus norvegicus) in a water maze learn both an egocentric trajectory and landmarks.
    Tamara C; Timberlake W; Leffel J
    J Comp Psychol; 2010 Aug; 124(3):302-16. PubMed ID: 20695662
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Hierarchical structures: chunking by food type facilitates spatial memory.
    Dallal NL; Meck WH
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 1990 Jan; 16(1):69-84. PubMed ID: 2303795
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Route and landmark learning by rats searching for food.
    Tamara C; Timberlake W
    Behav Processes; 2011 Jan; 86(1):125-32. PubMed ID: 21044664
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Landmark-based search memory in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris).
    Fiset S
    J Comp Psychol; 2007 Nov; 121(4):345-53. PubMed ID: 18085917
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Overshadowing between landmarks on the touchscreen and in arena with pigeons.
    Leising KJ; Garlick D; Blaisdell AP
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2011 Oct; 37(4):488-94. PubMed ID: 21604908
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Global and local spatial landmarks: their role during foraging by Columbian ground squirrels (Spermophilus columbianus).
    Vlasak AN
    Anim Cogn; 2006 Jan; 9(1):71-80. PubMed ID: 16163480
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Gender differences in landmark learning for virtual navigation: the role of distance to a goal.
    Chamizo VD; Artigas AA; Sansa J; Banterla F
    Behav Processes; 2011 Sep; 88(1):20-6. PubMed ID: 21736927
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Landmark use by Clark's nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana): influence of disorientation and cue rotation on distance and direction estimates.
    Kelly DM; Kamil AC; Cheng K
    Anim Cogn; 2010 Jan; 13(1):175-88. PubMed ID: 19579038
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Blocking of spatial control by landmarks in rats.
    Stahlman WD; Blaisdell AP
    Behav Processes; 2009 May; 81(1):114-8. PubMed ID: 19429205
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 19.