These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

195 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 8990972)

  • 1. Modularity and development: the case of spatial reorientation.
    Hermer L; Spelke E
    Cognition; 1996 Dec; 61(3):195-232. PubMed ID: 8990972
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Modularity as a fish (Xenotoca eiseni) views it: conjoining geometric and nongeometric information for spatial reorientation.
    Sovrano VA; Bisazza A; Vallortigara G
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2003 Jul; 29(3):199-210. PubMed ID: 12884679
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Modularity and spatial reorientation in a simple mind: encoding of geometric and nongeometric properties of a spatial environment by fish.
    Sovrano VA; Bisazza A; Vallortigara G
    Cognition; 2002 Sep; 85(2):B51-9. PubMed ID: 12127704
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Separate geometric and non-geometric modules for spatial reorientation: evidence from a lopsided animal brain.
    Vallortigara G; Pagni P; Sovrano VA
    J Cogn Neurosci; 2004 Apr; 16(3):390-400. PubMed ID: 15072675
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Sources of flexibility in human cognition: dual-task studies of space and language.
    Hermer-Vazquez L; Spelke ES; Katsnelson AS
    Cogn Psychol; 1999 Aug; 39(1):3-36. PubMed ID: 10433786
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children.
    Hermer L; Spelke ES
    Nature; 1994 Jul; 370(6484):57-9. PubMed ID: 8015605
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Language, space, and the development of cognitive flexibility in humans: the case of two spatial memory tasks.
    Hermer-Vazquez L; Moffet A; Munkholm P
    Cognition; 2001 May; 79(3):263-99. PubMed ID: 11165214
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Children reorient using the left/right sense of coloured landmarks at 18-24 months.
    Nardini M; Atkinson J; Burgess N
    Cognition; 2008 Jan; 106(1):519-27. PubMed ID: 17379204
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Rhesus monkeys use geometric and nongeometric information during a reorientation task.
    Gouteux S; Thinus-Blanc C; Vauclair J
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2001 Sep; 130(3):505-19. PubMed ID: 11561924
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Reorientation by geometric and landmark information in environments of different size.
    Vallortigara G; Feruglio M; Sovrano VA
    Dev Sci; 2005 Sep; 8(5):393-401. PubMed ID: 16048511
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Children's use of geometry for reorientation.
    Lee SA; Spelke ES
    Dev Sci; 2008 Sep; 11(5):743-9. PubMed ID: 18801130
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Instructional and contextual effects on eternal memory strategy use in young children.
    Fletcher KL; Bray NW
    J Exp Child Psychol; 1997 Nov; 67(2):204-22. PubMed ID: 9388806
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Young children reorient by computing layout geometry, not by matching images of the environment.
    Lee SA; Spelke ES
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2011 Feb; 18(1):192-8. PubMed ID: 21327347
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Children's use of geometry and landmarks to reorient in an open space.
    Gouteux S; Spelke ES
    Cognition; 2001 Sep; 81(2):119-48. PubMed ID: 11376639
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? Squaring theory and evidence.
    Cheng K; Newcombe NS
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2005 Feb; 12(1):1-23. PubMed ID: 15945200
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Children's questions: a mechanism for cognitive development.
    Chouinard MM
    Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 2007; 72(1):vii-ix, 1-112; discussion 113-26. PubMed ID: 17394580
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Children's use of landmarks: implications for modularity theory.
    Learmonth AE; Nadel L; Newcombe NS
    Psychol Sci; 2002 Jul; 13(4):337-41. PubMed ID: 12137136
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Toddlers' use of metric information and landmarks to reorient.
    Learmonth AE; Newcombe NS; Huttenlocher J
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2001 Nov; 80(3):225-44. PubMed ID: 11583524
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Navigation as a source of geometric knowledge: young children's use of length, angle, distance, and direction in a reorientation task.
    Lee SA; Sovrano VA; Spelke ES
    Cognition; 2012 Apr; 123(1):144-61. PubMed ID: 22257573
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Is language necessary for human spatial reorientation? Reconsidering evidence from dual task paradigms.
    Ratliff KR; Newcombe NS
    Cogn Psychol; 2008 Mar; 56(2):142-63. PubMed ID: 17663986
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 10.