These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

188 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 9050392)

  • 1. Children's differential performance on deductive and inductive syllogisms.
    Galotti KM; Komatsu LK; Voelz S
    Dev Psychol; 1997 Jan; 33(1):70-8. PubMed ID: 9050392
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Children's and adults' evaluation of the certainty of deductive inferences, inductive inferences, and guesses.
    Pillow BH
    Child Dev; 2002; 73(3):779-92. PubMed ID: 12038551
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Children's and adults' judgments of the certainty of deductive inferences, inductive inferences, and guesses.
    Pillow BH; Pearson RM; Hecht M; Bremer A
    J Genet Psychol; 2010; 171(3):203-17. PubMed ID: 20836430
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Proof construction: adolescent development from inductive to deductive problem-solving strategies.
    Foltz C; Overton WF; Ricco RB
    J Exp Child Psychol; 1995 Apr; 59(2):179-95. PubMed ID: 7722434
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Preadolescents Solve Natural Syllogisms Proficiently.
    Politzer G; Bosc-Miné C; Sander E
    Cogn Sci; 2017 May; 41 Suppl 5():1031-1061. PubMed ID: 26991789
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Exploring how students think: a new method combining think-aloud and concept mapping protocols.
    Pottier P; Hardouin JB; Hodges BD; Pistorius MA; Connault J; Durant C; Clairand R; Sebille V; Barrier JH; Planchon B
    Med Educ; 2010 Sep; 44(9):926-935. PubMed ID: 20716103
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Representation and reasoning in early numerical development: counting, conservation, and comparisons between sets.
    Sophian C
    Child Dev; 1995 Apr; 66(2):559-77. PubMed ID: 7750384
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Representations as mediators of adolescent deductive reasoning.
    Klaczynski PA; Narasimham G
    Dev Psychol; 1998 Sep; 34(5):865-81. PubMed ID: 9779734
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Narrative comprehension in 4-7-year-old children with autism: testing the Weak Central Coherence account.
    Nuske HJ; Bavin EL
    Int J Lang Commun Disord; 2011; 46(1):108-19. PubMed ID: 20565231
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Setters and samoyeds: the emergence of subordinate level categories as a basis for inductive inference in preschool-age children.
    Waxman SR; Lynch EB; Casey KL; Baer L
    Dev Psychol; 1997 Nov; 33(6):1074-90. PubMed ID: 9383629
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Reasoning processes in clinical reasoning: from the perspective of cognitive psychology.
    Shin HS
    Korean J Med Educ; 2019 Dec; 31(4):299-308. PubMed ID: 31813196
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Understanding inference as a source of knowledge: children's ability to evaluate the certainty of deduction, perception, and guessing.
    Pillow BH; Hill V; Boyce A; Stein C
    Dev Psychol; 2000 Mar; 36(2):169-79. PubMed ID: 10749074
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Exploring the macrostructure of children's central conceptual structures in the domains of number and narrative.
    Case R; Okamoto Y; Henderson B; McKeough A; Bleiker C
    Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 1996; 61(1-2):59-82. PubMed ID: 8657169
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Is inferential reasoning just probabilistic reasoning in disguise?
    Markovits H; Handley S
    Mem Cognit; 2005 Oct; 33(7):1315-23. PubMed ID: 16532862
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Diversity-based reasoning in children.
    Heit E; Hahn U
    Cogn Psychol; 2001 Dec; 43(4):243-73. PubMed ID: 11741343
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Children's understanding of class inclusion hierarchies: the relationship between external representation and task performance.
    Greene TR
    J Exp Child Psychol; 1989 Aug; 48(1):62-89. PubMed ID: 2754389
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Occam's rattle: children's use of simplicity and probability to constrain inference.
    Bonawitz EB; Lombrozo T
    Dev Psychol; 2012 Jul; 48(4):1156-64. PubMed ID: 22201450
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. The dimensionality of reasoning: Inductive and deductive inference can be explained by a single process.
    Hayes BK; Stephens RG; Ngo J; Dunn JC
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2018 Sep; 44(9):1333-1351. PubMed ID: 29389195
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Continuity and change in the development of category-based induction: The test case of diversity-based reasoning.
    Rhodes M; Liebenson P
    Cogn Psychol; 2015 Nov; 82():74-95. PubMed ID: 26379264
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Young children's understanding of conflicting mental representation predicts suggestibility.
    Welch-Ross MK; Diecidue K; Miller SA
    Dev Psychol; 1997 Jan; 33(1):43-53. PubMed ID: 9050389
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 10.