These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

123 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 9156215)

  • 1. Perception of moving lines: interactions between local perpendicular signals and 2D motion signals.
    Castet E; Wuerger S
    Vision Res; 1997 Mar; 37(6):705-20. PubMed ID: 9156215
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. The extrinsic/intrinsic classification of two-dimensional motion signals with barber-pole stimuli.
    Castet E; Charton V; Dufour A
    Vision Res; 1999 Mar; 39(5):915-32. PubMed ID: 10341945
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Action and attentional load can influence aperture effects on motion perception.
    DeLucia PR; Ott TE
    Exp Brain Res; 2011 Mar; 209(2):215-24. PubMed ID: 21267553
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Perceived speed of moving lines depends on orientation, length, speed and luminance.
    Castet E; Lorenceau J; Shiffrar M; Bonnet C
    Vision Res; 1993 Sep; 33(14):1921-36. PubMed ID: 8249311
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. The cyclopean (stereoscopic) barber pole illusion.
    Patterson R; Bowd C; Donnelly M
    Vision Res; 1998 Jul; 38(14):2119-25. PubMed ID: 9797972
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Lines and dots: characteristics of the motion integration process.
    Lamouret I; Cornilleau-Pérès V; Droulez J
    Vision Res; 2001 Aug; 41(17):2207-19. PubMed ID: 11448713
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Effects of a static textured background on motion integration.
    Lorenceau J; Boucart M
    Vision Res; 1995 Aug; 35(16):2303-14. PubMed ID: 7571466
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Speed decrements are seen better than increments through small apertures.
    Hohnsbein J; Mateeff S
    Exp Brain Res; 2002 Feb; 142(3):434-8. PubMed ID: 11819053
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. The aperture problem--I. Perception of nonrigidity and motion direction in translating sinusoidal lines.
    Nakayama K; Silverman GH
    Vision Res; 1988; 28(6):739-46. PubMed ID: 3227650
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Pattern cues disambiguate perceived direction in simple moving stimuli.
    Badcock DR; McKendrick AM; Ma-Wyatt A
    Vision Res; 2003 Oct; 43(22):2291-301. PubMed ID: 12962987
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Saliency from orthogonal velocity component in texture segregation.
    Casco C; Grieco A; Giora E; Martinelli M
    Vision Res; 2006 Mar; 46(6-7):1091-8. PubMed ID: 16289199
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Differential processing: towards a unified model of direction and speed perception.
    Farrell-Whelan M; Brooks KR
    Vision Res; 2013 Nov; 92():10-8. PubMed ID: 23994486
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Stereoscopic occlusion and the aperture problem for motion: a new solution.
    Anderson BL
    Vision Res; 1999 Apr; 39(7):1273-84. PubMed ID: 10343841
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Speed gradients and the perception of surface slant: analysis is two-dimensional not one-dimensional.
    Meese TS; Harris MG; Freeman TC
    Vision Res; 1995 Oct; 35(20):2879-88. PubMed ID: 8533327
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Aging and the Visual Perception of Motion Direction: Solving the Aperture Problem.
    Shain LM; Norman JF
    Perception; 2018 Jul; 47(7):735-750. PubMed ID: 29783919
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. The predictive power of trajectory motion.
    Watamaniuk SN
    Vision Res; 2005 Nov; 45(24):2993-3003. PubMed ID: 16153677
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Occlusion and the solution to the aperture problem for motion.
    Shimojo S; Silverman GH; Nakayama K
    Vision Res; 1989; 29(5):619-26. PubMed ID: 2603398
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Integration of one- and two-dimensional motion signals in infants: evidence from the barber-pole illusion.
    Dobkins KR; Lewis LB; Fine I
    Vision Res; 2006 Oct; 46(20):3360-72. PubMed ID: 16854447
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. The directional tuning of the barber-pole illusion.
    Fisher N; Zanker JM
    Perception; 2001; 30(11):1321-36. PubMed ID: 11768487
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Prior depth information can bias motion perception.
    Graf EW; Adams WJ; Lages M
    J Vis; 2004 May; 4(6):427-33. PubMed ID: 15330710
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.