These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

141 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 9337629)

  • 1. Rethinking infant knowledge: toward an adaptive process account of successes and failures in object permanence tasks.
    Munakata Y; McClelland JL; Johnson MH; Siegler RS
    Psychol Rev; 1997 Oct; 104(4):686-713. PubMed ID: 9337629
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Factors affecting infants' manual search for occluded objects and the genesis of object permanence.
    Moore MK; Meltzoff AN
    Infant Behav Dev; 2008 Apr; 31(2):168-80. PubMed ID: 18036668
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Object permanence and method of disappearance: looking measures further contradict reaching measures.
    Charles EP; Rivera SM
    Dev Sci; 2009 Nov; 12(6):991-1006. PubMed ID: 19840053
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Individual differences in object permanence performance at 8 months: locomotor experience and brain electrical activity.
    Bell MA; Fox NA
    Dev Psychobiol; 1997 Dec; 31(4):287-97. PubMed ID: 9413676
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Rich interpretation vs. deflationary accounts in cognitive development: the case of means-end skills in 7-month-old infants.
    Munakata Y; Bauer D; Stackhouse T; Landgraf L; Huddleston J
    Cognition; 2002 Apr; 83(3):B43-53. PubMed ID: 11934406
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Familiarity breeds searching: infants reverse their novelty preferences when reaching for hidden objects.
    Shinskey JL; Munakata Y
    Psychol Sci; 2005 Aug; 16(8):596-600. PubMed ID: 16102061
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Something old, something new: a developmental transition from familiarity to novelty preferences with hidden objects.
    Shinskey JL; Munakata Y
    Dev Sci; 2010 Mar; 13(2):378-84. PubMed ID: 20136935
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Inability of five-month-old infants to retrieve a contiguous object: a failure of conceptual understanding or of control of action?
    Diamond A; Lee EY
    Child Dev; 2000; 71(6):1477-94. PubMed ID: 11194250
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Detecting continuity violations in infancy: a new account and new evidence from covering and tube events.
    Wang SH; Baillargeon R; Paterson S
    Cognition; 2005 Mar; 95(2):129-73. PubMed ID: 15694644
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Infants' comprehension of toy replicas as symbols for real objects.
    Younger BA; Johnson KE
    Cogn Psychol; 2004 Mar; 48(2):207-42. PubMed ID: 14732411
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. The sound of darkness: why do auditory cues aid infants' search for objects hidden by darkness but not by visible occluders?
    Shinskey JL
    Dev Psychol; 2008 Nov; 44(6):1715-25. PubMed ID: 18999333
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. A unified computational model of the development of object unity, object permanence, and occluded object trajectory perception.
    Franz A; Triesch J
    Infant Behav Dev; 2010 Dec; 33(4):635-53. PubMed ID: 20864178
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Representing occluded objects in the human infant brain.
    Kaufman J; Csibra G; Johnson MH
    Proc Biol Sci; 2003 Nov; 270 Suppl 2(Suppl 2):S140-3. PubMed ID: 14667363
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Priming 4.5-month-old infants to use height information by enhancing retrieval.
    Wang SH
    Dev Psychol; 2011 Jan; 47(1):26-38. PubMed ID: 21058828
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Performance on a stage IV object-permanence task with standard and nonstandard covers.
    Rader N; Spiro DJ; Firestone PB
    Child Dev; 1979 Sep; 50(3):908-10. PubMed ID: 498862
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Development of means-end behavior in young infants: pulling a support to retrieve a distant object.
    Willatts P
    Dev Psychol; 1999 May; 35(3):651-67. PubMed ID: 10380857
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Sound effects: Multimodal input helps infants find displaced objects.
    Shinskey JL
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2017 Sep; 35(3):317-333. PubMed ID: 27868211
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Detecting Transparent Barriers: Clear Evidence Against the Means-End Deficit Account of Search Failures.
    Shinskey JL; Munakata Y
    Infancy; 2001 Jul; 2(3):395-404. PubMed ID: 33451212
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. 2.5-month-old infants' reasoning about when objects should and should not be occluded.
    Aguiar A; Baillargeon R
    Cogn Psychol; 1999 Sep; 39(2):116-57. PubMed ID: 10462457
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Why do young infants fail to search for hidden objects?
    Baillargeon R; Graber M; Devos J; Black J
    Cognition; 1990 Sep; 36(3):255-84. PubMed ID: 2265529
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.