These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

273 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 9440301)

  • 1. Integrating relationship constructs and emotional experience into false belief tasks in preschool children.
    Symons D; McLaughlin E; Moore C; Morine S
    J Exp Child Psychol; 1997 Dec; 67(3):423-47. PubMed ID: 9440301
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Five-year-old children's difficulty with false belief when the sought entity is a person.
    Rai R; Mitchell P
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2004 Oct; 89(2):112-26. PubMed ID: 15388301
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Belief-desire reasoning as a process of selection.
    Leslie AM; German TP; Polizzi P
    Cogn Psychol; 2005 Feb; 50(1):45-85. PubMed ID: 15556129
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. The relationship between second-order false belief and display rules reasoning: the integration of cognitive and affective social understanding.
    Naito M; Seki Y
    Dev Sci; 2009 Jan; 12(1):150-64. PubMed ID: 19120423
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Conceptual coherence in the child's theory of mind: training children to understand belief.
    Slaughter V; Gopnik A
    Child Dev; 1996 Dec; 67(6):2967-88. PubMed ID: 9071768
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Understanding theory of mind in children who are deaf.
    Marschark M; Green V; Hindmarsh G; Walker S
    J Child Psychol Psychiatry; 2000 Nov; 41(8):1067-73. PubMed ID: 11099123
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Children's attention to beliefs in interactive persuasion tasks.
    Bartsch K; London K; Campbell MD
    Dev Psychol; 2007 Jan; 43(1):111-20. PubMed ID: 17201512
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Social cognition is not reducible to theory of mind: when children use deontic rules to predict the behaviour of others.
    Clément F; Bernard S; Kaufmann L
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2011 Nov; 29(Pt 4):910-28. PubMed ID: 21995744
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. The role of strategic planning in accessing false-belief understanding.
    Hala S; Chandler M
    Child Dev; 1996 Dec; 67(6):2948-66. PubMed ID: 9071767
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Metacognitive development of deaf children: lessons from the appearance-reality and false belief tasks.
    Courtin C; Melot AM
    Dev Sci; 2005 Jan; 8(1):16-25. PubMed ID: 15647063
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Young children's attribution of action to beliefs and desires.
    Bartsch K; Wellman H
    Child Dev; 1989 Aug; 60(4):946-64. PubMed ID: 2758888
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Health care utilization and needs after pediatric traumatic brain injury.
    Slomine BS; McCarthy ML; Ding R; MacKenzie EJ; Jaffe KM; Aitken ME; Durbin DR; Christensen JR; Dorsch AM; Paidas CN;
    Pediatrics; 2006 Apr; 117(4):e663-74. PubMed ID: 16533894
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Gesture as a window on children's beginning understanding of false belief.
    Carlson SM; Wong A; Lemke M; Cosser C
    Child Dev; 2005; 76(1):73-86. PubMed ID: 15693758
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Competence and performance in belief-desire reasoning across two cultures: the truth, the whole truth and nothing but the truth about false belief?
    Yazdi AA; German TP; Defeyter MA; Siegal M
    Cognition; 2006 Jun; 100(2):343-68. PubMed ID: 16087170
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Seeing it my way: a case of a selective deficit in inhibiting self-perspective.
    Samson D; Apperly IA; Kathirgamanathan U; Humphreys GW
    Brain; 2005 May; 128(Pt 5):1102-11. PubMed ID: 15774506
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Two sources of evidence on the non-automaticity of true and false belief ascription.
    Back E; Apperly IA
    Cognition; 2010 Apr; 115(1):54-70. PubMed ID: 20060965
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Children's understanding of the modal expression of speaker certainty and uncertainty and its relation to the development of a representational theory of mind.
    Moore C; Pure K; Furrow D
    Child Dev; 1990 Jun; 61(3):722-30. PubMed ID: 2364747
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Social influences on false belief access: specific sibling influences or general apprenticeship?
    Lewis C; Freeman NH; Kyriakidou C; Maridaki-Kassotaki K; Berridge DM
    Child Dev; 1996 Dec; 67(6):2930-47. PubMed ID: 9071766
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Theory of mind development in deaf children: a nonverbal test of false-belief understanding.
    Figueras-Costa B; Harris P
    J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ; 2001; 6(2):92-102. PubMed ID: 15451854
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. The relation between individual differences in fantasy and theory of mind.
    Taylor M; Carlson SM
    Child Dev; 1997 Jun; 68(3):436-55. PubMed ID: 9249959
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 14.