These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


PUBMED FOR HANDHELDS

Journal Abstract Search


241 related items for PubMed ID: 11394929

  • 1.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 2. Intermodal learning in infancy: learning on the basis of two kinds of invariant relations in audible and visible events.
    Bahrick LE.
    Child Dev; 1988 Feb; 59(1):197-209. PubMed ID: 3342712
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 3. Intersensory redundancy facilitates discrimination of tempo in 3-month-old infants.
    Bahrick LE, Flom R, Lickliter R.
    Dev Psychobiol; 2002 Dec; 41(4):352-63. PubMed ID: 12430159
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 4. Infant perception of audio-visual speech synchrony.
    Lewkowicz DJ.
    Dev Psychol; 2010 Jan; 46(1):66-77. PubMed ID: 20053007
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 5. Learning of syllable-object relations by preverbal infants: the role of temporal synchrony and syllable distinctiveness.
    Gogate LJ.
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2010 Mar; 105(3):178-97. PubMed ID: 20004909
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 6.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 7. Perception of audiovisual rhythm and its invariance in 4- to 10-month-old infants.
    Lewkowicz DJ, Marcovitch S.
    Dev Psychobiol; 2006 May; 48(4):288-300. PubMed ID: 16617468
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 8.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 9.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 10.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 11. Two-month-old infants' sensitivity to changes in arbitrary syllable-object pairings: the role of temporal synchrony.
    Gogate LJ, Prince CG, Matatyaho DJ.
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2009 Apr; 35(2):508-19. PubMed ID: 19331504
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 12.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 13.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 14. Developmental change in infants' perception of correlations among attributes.
    Younger BA, Cohen LB.
    Child Dev; 1986 Jun; 57(3):803-15. PubMed ID: 3720405
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 15. Preterm and term infants’ perception of temporally coordinated syllable–object pairings: implications for lexical development.
    Gogate L, Maganti M, Perenyi A.
    J Speech Lang Hear Res; 2014 Feb; 57(1):187-98. PubMed ID: 24023374
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 16.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 17. Infants' sensitivity to correlations between static and dynamic features in a category context.
    Rakison DH.
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2004 Sep; 89(1):1-30. PubMed ID: 15336916
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 18. Auditory dominance and its change in the course of development.
    Robinson CW, Sloutsky VM.
    Child Dev; 2004 Sep; 75(5):1387-401. PubMed ID: 15369521
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 19. Infants' timing strategies to optical collisions: a longitudinal study.
    Kayed NS, van der Meer AL.
    Infant Behav Dev; 2007 Feb; 30(1):50-9. PubMed ID: 17292779
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 20.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]


    Page: [Next] [New Search]
    of 13.