These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


PUBMED FOR HANDHELDS

Journal Abstract Search


149 related items for PubMed ID: 15615317

  • 1. Illusory recollection of voices.
    Roediger HL, McDermott KB, Pisoni DB, Gallo DA.
    Memory; 2004 Sep; 12(5):586-602. PubMed ID: 15615317
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 2. Examining the basis for illusory recollection: the role of remember/know instructions.
    Geraci L, McCabe DP.
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2006 Jun; 13(3):466-73. PubMed ID: 17048732
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 3.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 4. Can test list context manipulations improve recognition accuracy in the DRM paradigm?
    Gunter RW, Ivanko SL, Bodner GE.
    Memory; 2005 Nov; 13(8):862-73. PubMed ID: 16298893
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 5. Modality effect in false recognition: evidence from Chinese characters.
    Mao WB, Yang ZL, Wang LS.
    Int J Psychol; 2010 Feb; 45(1):4-11. PubMed ID: 22043843
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 6.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 7.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 8.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 9. Valence and the development of immediate and long-term false memory illusions.
    Howe ML, Candel I, Otgaar H, Malone C, Wimmer MC.
    Memory; 2010 Jan; 18(1):58-75. PubMed ID: 20391177
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 10. The difference between implicit and explicit associative processes at study in creating false memory in the DRM paradigm.
    Kawasaki Y, Yama H.
    Memory; 2006 Jan; 14(1):68-78. PubMed ID: 16423743
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 11. Affective valence influences participant's susceptibility to false memories and illusory recollection.
    Dehon H, Larøi F, Van der Linden M.
    Emotion; 2010 Oct; 10(5):627-39. PubMed ID: 21038946
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 12. Re-exposure to studied items at test does not influence false recognition.
    Dodd MD, Sheard ED, MacLeod CM.
    Memory; 2006 Jan; 14(1):115-26. PubMed ID: 16423748
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 13. False memories in children and adults: age, distinctiveness, and subjective experience.
    Ghetti S, Qin J, Goodman GS.
    Dev Psychol; 2002 Sep; 38(5):705-18. PubMed ID: 12220049
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 14. Pictorial encoding effects and memory confusions in the Deese-Roediger-McDermott paradigm: evidence for the activation of spontaneous imagery.
    Foley MA, Foy J.
    Memory; 2008 Oct; 16(7):712-27. PubMed ID: 18720219
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 15. The effects of associations and aging on illusory recollection.
    Gallo DA, Roediger HL.
    Mem Cognit; 2003 Oct; 31(7):1036-44. PubMed ID: 14704018
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 16. What factors underlie associative and categorical memory illusions? The roles of backward associative strength and interitem connectivity.
    Knott LM, Dewhurst SA, Howe ML.
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2012 Jan; 38(1):229-39. PubMed ID: 21875250
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 17. Mood and the DRM paradigm: An investigation of the effects of valence and arousal on false memory.
    Van Damme I.
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2013 Jun; 66(6):1060-81. PubMed ID: 23057583
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 18.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 19.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 20.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]


    Page: [Next] [New Search]
    of 8.