These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


PUBMED FOR HANDHELDS

Journal Abstract Search


247 related items for PubMed ID: 16941155

  • 1. Multiple landmarks, the encoding of environmental geometry and the spatial logics of a dual brain.
    Chiesa AD, Pecchia T, Tommasi L, Vallortigara G.
    Anim Cogn; 2006 Oct; 9(4):281-93. PubMed ID: 16941155
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 2. Spatial cognition based on geometry and landmarks in the domestic chick (Gallus gallus).
    Chiesa AD, Speranza M, Tommasi L, Vallortigara G.
    Behav Brain Res; 2006 Nov 25; 175(1):119-27. PubMed ID: 16979247
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 3. Encoding of geometric and landmark information in the left and right hemispheres of the Avian Brain.
    Tommasi L, Vallortigara G.
    Behav Neurosci; 2001 Jun 25; 115(3):602-13. PubMed ID: 11439449
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 4. Hemispheric processing of landmark and geometric information in male and female domestic chicks (Gallus gallus).
    Tommasi L, Vallortigara G.
    Behav Brain Res; 2004 Nov 05; 155(1):85-96. PubMed ID: 15325782
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 5. Separate geometric and non-geometric modules for spatial reorientation: evidence from a lopsided animal brain.
    Vallortigara G, Pagni P, Sovrano VA.
    J Cogn Neurosci; 2004 Apr 05; 16(3):390-400. PubMed ID: 15072675
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 6. Representation of two geometric features of the environment in the domestic chick ( Gallus gallus).
    Tommasi L, Polli C.
    Anim Cogn; 2004 Jan 05; 7(1):53-9. PubMed ID: 12884079
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 7. Reorienting strategies in a rectangular array of landmarks by domestic chicks (Gallus gallus).
    Pecchia T, Vallortigara G.
    J Comp Psychol; 2010 May 05; 124(2):147-58. PubMed ID: 20476814
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 8. Spatial reorientation: the effects of space size on the encoding of landmark and geometry information.
    Chiandetti C, Regolin L, Sovrano VA, Vallortigara G.
    Anim Cogn; 2007 Apr 05; 10(2):159-68. PubMed ID: 17136416
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 9. How do global and local geometries shape exploratory behavior in rats?
    Yaski O, Eilam D.
    Behav Brain Res; 2008 Mar 05; 187(2):334-42. PubMed ID: 17988750
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 10.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 11.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 12.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 13.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 14.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 15.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 16. Global and local spatial landmarks: their role during foraging by Columbian ground squirrels (Spermophilus columbianus).
    Vlasak AN.
    Anim Cogn; 2006 Jan 05; 9(1):71-80. PubMed ID: 16163480
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 17. Effects of light stimulation of embryos on the use of position-specific and object-specific cues in binocular and monocular domestic chicks (Gallus gallus).
    Chiandetti C, Regolin L, Rogers LJ, Vallortigara G.
    Behav Brain Res; 2005 Aug 30; 163(1):10-7. PubMed ID: 15927280
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 18. Searching in the center: pigeons (Columba livid) encode relative distance from walls of an enclosure.
    Gray ER, Spetch ML, Kelly DM, Nguyen A.
    J Comp Psychol; 2004 Mar 30; 118(1):113-7. PubMed ID: 15008679
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 19.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 20.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]


    Page: [Next] [New Search]
    of 13.