These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


PUBMED FOR HANDHELDS

Journal Abstract Search


319 related items for PubMed ID: 18766389

  • 1. Means to an end: neotropical parrots manage to pull strings to meet their goals.
    Schuck-Paim C, Borsari A, Ottoni EB.
    Anim Cogn; 2009 Mar; 12(2):287-301. PubMed ID: 18766389
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 2. Big brains are not enough: performance of three parrot species in the trap-tube paradigm.
    Liedtke J, Werdenich D, Gajdon GK, Huber L, Wanker R.
    Anim Cogn; 2011 Jan; 14(1):143-9. PubMed ID: 20814805
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 3. Means-end comprehension in four parrot species: explained by social complexity.
    Krasheninnikova A, Bräger S, Wanker R.
    Anim Cogn; 2013 Sep; 16(5):755-64. PubMed ID: 23397182
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 4.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 5. Bajan Birds Pull Strings: Two Wild Antillean Species Enter the Select Club of String-Pullers.
    Audet JN, Ducatez S, Lefebvre L.
    PLoS One; 2016 Sep; 11(8):e0156112. PubMed ID: 27533282
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 6. Domestic cats (Felis catus) do not show causal understanding in a string-pulling task.
    Whitt E, Douglas M, Osthaus B, Hocking I.
    Anim Cogn; 2009 Sep; 12(5):739-43. PubMed ID: 19449193
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 7.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 8. Technical intelligence in animals: the kea model.
    Huber L, Gajdon GK.
    Anim Cogn; 2006 Oct; 9(4):295-305. PubMed ID: 16909237
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 9. Means-means-end tool choice in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus): finding the limits on primates' knowledge of tools.
    Santos LR, Rosati A, Sproul C, Spaulding B, Hauser MD.
    Anim Cogn; 2005 Oct; 8(4):236-46. PubMed ID: 15668762
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 10. Mirror-mediated string-pulling task in Eurasian jays (Garrulus glandarius).
    Baciadonna L, Cornero FM, Clayton NS, Emery NJ.
    Anim Cogn; 2022 Jun; 25(3):691-700. PubMed ID: 34913110
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 11.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 12. Testing problem solving in turkey vultures (Cathartes aura) using the string-pulling test.
    Ellison AM, Watson J, Demers E.
    Anim Cogn; 2015 Jan; 18(1):111-8. PubMed ID: 25015133
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 13. Spontaneous performance of wild baboons on three novel food-access puzzles.
    Laidre ME.
    Anim Cogn; 2008 Apr; 11(2):223-30. PubMed ID: 17710453
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 14.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 15. [Corvidae are able to understand the logical structure in string-pulling tasks].
    Bagotskaia MS, Smirnova AA, Zorina ZA.
    Zh Vyssh Nerv Deiat Im I P Pavlova; 2010 Apr; 60(5):543-51. PubMed ID: 21260978
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 16.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 17. Non-tool-using rooks, Corvus frugilegus, solve the trap-tube problem.
    Tebbich S, Seed AM, Emery NJ, Clayton NS.
    Anim Cogn; 2007 Apr; 10(2):225-31. PubMed ID: 17171360
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 18.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 19. Performance of blue-fronted amazon parrots (Amazona aestiva) when solving the pebbles-and-seeds and multi-access-box paradigms: ex situ and in situ experiments.
    Godinho L, Marinho Y, Bezerra B.
    Anim Cogn; 2020 May; 23(3):455-464. PubMed ID: 32034538
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 20.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]


    Page: [Next] [New Search]
    of 16.