These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
Pubmed for Handhelds
PUBMED FOR HANDHELDS
Journal Abstract Search
540 related items for PubMed ID: 19027776
1. Slant cue are combined early in visual processing: evidence from visual search. Sousa R, Brenner E, Smeets JB. Vision Res; 2009 Jan; 49(2):257-61. PubMed ID: 19027776 [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
10. Disparity-defined objects moving in depth do not elicit three-dimensional shape constancy. Scarfe P, Hibbard PB. Vision Res; 2006 May; 46(10):1599-610. PubMed ID: 16364392 [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
11. The role of (micro)saccades and blinks in perceptual bi-stability from slant rivalry. van Dam LC, van Ee R. Vision Res; 2005 Aug; 45(18):2417-35. PubMed ID: 15894347 [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
12. Stereoscopic depth aftereffects without retinal position correspondence between adaptation and test stimuli. Taya S, Sato M, Nakamizo S. Vision Res; 2005 Jun; 45(14):1857-66. PubMed ID: 15797775 [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
13. The perceived visual direction of monocular objects in random-dot stereograms is influenced by perceived depth and allelotropia. Hariharan-Vilupuru S, Bedell HE. Vision Res; 2009 Jan; 49(2):190-201. PubMed ID: 18992271 [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
14. Slant or occlusion: global factors resolve stereoscopic ambiguity in sets of horizontal lines. Gillam B, Grove PM. Vision Res; 2004 Jan; 44(20):2359-66. PubMed ID: 15246752 [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
17. Effect of a disparity pattern on the perception of direction: non-retinal information masks retinal information. Ishii M. Vision Res; 2009 Jun; 49(12):1563-8. PubMed ID: 19324065 [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]