These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


PUBMED FOR HANDHELDS

Journal Abstract Search


631 related items for PubMed ID: 19324066

  • 1. Location and color biases have different influences on selective attention.
    Fecteau JH, Korjoukov I, Roelfsema PR.
    Vision Res; 2009 May; 49(9):996-1005. PubMed ID: 19324066
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 2. Not all features are created equal: Processing asymmetries between location and object features.
    Chen Z.
    Vision Res; 2009 May; 49(11):1481-91. PubMed ID: 19303423
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 3. Priming of Pop-out provides reliable measures of target activation and distractor inhibition in selective attention.
    Lamy D, Antebi C, Aviani N, Carmel T.
    Vision Res; 2008 Jan; 48(1):30-41. PubMed ID: 18054983
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 4. The remains of the trial: goal-determined inter-trial suppression of selective attention.
    Lleras A, Levinthal BR, Kawahara J.
    Prog Brain Res; 2009 Jan; 176():195-213. PubMed ID: 19733758
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 5. Magnocellular involvement in flanked-letter identification relates to the allocation of attention.
    Omtzigt D, Hendriks AW.
    Vision Res; 2004 Jan; 44(16):1927-40. PubMed ID: 15145686
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 6. The footprints of visual attention during search with 100% valid and 100% invalid cues.
    Eckstein MP, Pham BT, Shimozaki SS.
    Vision Res; 2004 Jun; 44(12):1193-207. PubMed ID: 15066385
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 7. Feature-based attention and the suppression of non-relevant object features.
    Wegener D, Ehn F, Aurich MK, Galashan FO, Kreiter AK.
    Vision Res; 2008 Dec; 48(27):2696-707. PubMed ID: 18824190
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 8. Processing multidimensional objects under different perceptual loads: the priority of bottom-up perceptual saliency.
    Wei P, Zhou X.
    Brain Res; 2006 Oct 09; 1114(1):113-24. PubMed ID: 16935270
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 9.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 10.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 11. Simultaneous priming along multiple feature dimensions in a visual search task.
    Kristjánsson A.
    Vision Res; 2006 Aug 09; 46(16):2554-70. PubMed ID: 16527323
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 12. Saccade trajectory deviations and inhibition-of-return: Measuring the amount of attentional processing.
    Theeuwes J, Van der Stigchel S.
    Vision Res; 2009 Jun 09; 49(10):1307-15. PubMed ID: 18723045
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 13.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 14. Priming of color and position during visual search in unilateral spatial neglect.
    Kristjánsson A, Vuilleumier P, Malhotra P, Husain M, Driver J.
    J Cogn Neurosci; 2005 Jun 09; 17(6):859-73. PubMed ID: 15969905
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 15.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 16.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 17.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 18.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 19. Dimension-based working memory-driven capture of visual selection.
    Pan Y, Xu B, Soto D.
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2009 Jun 09; 62(6):1123-31. PubMed ID: 19142832
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 20.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]


    Page: [Next] [New Search]
    of 32.