These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
6. Dingoes (Canis dingo) can use human social cues to locate hidden food. Smith BP, Litchfield CA. Anim Cogn; 2010 Mar; 13(2):367-76. PubMed ID: 19779743 [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
7. Learning and owner-stranger effects on interspecific communication in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). Elgier AM, Jakovcevic A, Mustaca AE, Bentosela M. Behav Processes; 2009 May; 81(1):44-9. PubMed ID: 19429195 [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
8. Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use a physical marker to locate hidden food. Riedel J, Buttelmann D, Call J, Tomasello M. Anim Cogn; 2006 Jan; 9(1):27-35. PubMed ID: 15846526 [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
9. Comprehension and utilisation of pointing gestures and gazing in dog-human communication in relatively complex situations. Lakatos G, Gácsi M, Topál J, Miklósi A. Anim Cogn; 2012 Mar; 15(2):201-13. PubMed ID: 21927851 [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
10. Communication between domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and humans: dogs are good learners. Elgier AM, Jakovcevic A, Barrera G, Mustaca AE, Bentosela M. Behav Processes; 2009 Jul; 81(3):402-8. PubMed ID: 19520240 [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
11. Can domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use referential emotional expressions to locate hidden food? Buttelmann D, Tomasello M. Anim Cogn; 2013 Jan; 16(1):137-45. PubMed ID: 22960805 [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
12. The response of guide dogs and pet dogs (Canis familiaris) to cues of human referential communication (pointing and gaze). Ittyerah M, Gaunet F. Anim Cogn; 2009 Mar; 12(2):257-65. PubMed ID: 18769948 [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
13. [Dangerous dogs in Berlin in comparison to the dog population--ways to reduce the dangerousness of dogs]. Kuhne F, Struwe R. Berl Munch Tierarztl Wochenschr; 2006 Mar; 119(11-12):445-55. PubMed ID: 17172131 [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
14. Making inferences about the location of hidden food: social dog, causal ape. Bräuer J, Kaminski J, Riedel J, Call J, Tomasello M. J Comp Psychol; 2006 Feb; 120(1):38-47. PubMed ID: 16551163 [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
15. The performance of bonobos (Pan paniscus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) in two versions of an object-choice task. Mulcahy NJ, Call J. J Comp Psychol; 2009 Aug; 123(3):304-9. PubMed ID: 19685972 [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
16. The use of experimenter-given cues by South African fur seals (Arctocephalus pusillus). Scheumann M, Call J. Anim Cogn; 2004 Oct; 7(4):224-30. PubMed ID: 15057598 [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
17. Cephalic index and perceived dog trainability. Helton WS. Behav Processes; 2009 Nov; 82(3):355-8. PubMed ID: 19683035 [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
18. A comparison of pet and purpose-bred research dog (Canis familiaris) performance on human-guided object-choice tasks. Lazarowski L, Dorman DC. Behav Processes; 2015 Jan; 110():60-7. PubMed ID: 25245303 [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]
20. The effect of development and individual differences in pointing comprehension of dogs. Gácsi M, Kara E, Belényi B, Topál J, Miklósi A. Anim Cogn; 2009 May; 12(3):471-9. PubMed ID: 19130102 [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related] Page: [Next] [New Search]