These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


PUBMED FOR HANDHELDS

Journal Abstract Search


170 related items for PubMed ID: 26114680

  • 1. Synchronous motion modulates animacy perception.
    Takahashi K, Watanabe K.
    J Vis; 2015; 15(8):17. PubMed ID: 26114680
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 2. The psychophysics of chasing: A case study in the perception of animacy.
    Gao T, Newman GE, Scholl BJ.
    Cogn Psychol; 2009 Sep; 59(2):154-79. PubMed ID: 19500784
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 3. Actual and illusory differences in constant speed influence the perception of animacy similarly.
    Szego PA, Rutherford MD.
    J Vis; 2007 Sep 18; 7(12):5.1-7. PubMed ID: 17997647
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 4. Detecting a trajectory embedded in random-direction motion noise.
    Watamaniuk SN, McKee SP, Grzywacz NM.
    Vision Res; 1995 Jan 18; 35(1):65-77. PubMed ID: 7839611
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 5. The predictive power of trajectory motion.
    Watamaniuk SN.
    Vision Res; 2005 Nov 18; 45(24):2993-3003. PubMed ID: 16153677
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 6. Perception of Animacy from the Motion of a Single Sound Object.
    Nielsen RH, Vuust P, Wallentin M.
    Perception; 2015 Feb 18; 44(2):183-97. PubMed ID: 26561971
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 7. Induced motion may account for the illusory transformation of optic flow fields found by Duffy and Wurtz.
    Meese TS, Smith V, Harris MG.
    Vision Res; 1995 Apr 18; 35(7):981-5. PubMed ID: 7762154
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 8. Displacement thresholds for coherent apparent motion in random dot-patterns.
    Ramachandran VS, Anstis SM.
    Vision Res; 1983 Apr 18; 23(12):1719-24. PubMed ID: 6666076
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 9.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 10. Perception of illusory occlusion in apparent motion.
    Ramachandran VS, Inada V, Kiama G.
    Vision Res; 1986 Apr 18; 26(10):1741-9. PubMed ID: 3617515
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 11.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 12.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 13. Motion-aftereffect-induced blindness.
    Lages M, Adams WJ, Graf EW.
    J Vis; 2009 Oct 13; 9(11):11.1-7. PubMed ID: 20053074
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 14. Apparent motion can be perceived between patterns with dissimilar spatial frequencies.
    Baro JA, Levinson E.
    Vision Res; 1988 Oct 13; 28(12):1311-3. PubMed ID: 3256148
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 15. Biological motion alters coherent motion perception.
    Fujimoto K, Yagi A.
    Perception; 2008 Oct 13; 37(12):1783-9. PubMed ID: 19227372
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 16.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 17. Effects of movement in the background field on long-range apparent motion.
    Finlay DC, Manning ML, Neill RA, Fenelon B.
    Vision Res; 1987 Oct 13; 27(9):1679-82. PubMed ID: 3445497
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 18. Visual inertia in apparent motion.
    Anstis S, Ramachandran VS.
    Vision Res; 1987 Oct 13; 27(5):755-64. PubMed ID: 3660637
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 19. Physical and biological constraints govern perceived animacy of scrambled human forms.
    Thurman SM, Lu H.
    Psychol Sci; 2013 Jul 01; 24(7):1133-41. PubMed ID: 23670885
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 20.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]


    Page: [Next] [New Search]
    of 9.