These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


PUBMED FOR HANDHELDS

Journal Abstract Search


180 related items for PubMed ID: 34913110

  • 1.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 2.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 3.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 4.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 5.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 6. Kea (Nestor notabilis) fail a loose-string connectivity task.
    Bastos APM, Wood PM, Taylor AH.
    Sci Rep; 2021 Jul 29; 11(1):15492. PubMed ID: 34326392
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 7. Vertical string-pulling in green jays (Cyanocorax yncas).
    Manrique HM, Molina AC, Posada S, Colell M.
    Behav Processes; 2017 Jul 29; 140():74-80. PubMed ID: 28412529
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 8.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 9.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 10. Exclusion in corvids: the performance of food-caching Eurasian jays (Garrulus glandarius).
    Shaw RC, Plotnik JM, Clayton NS.
    J Comp Psychol; 2013 Nov 29; 127(4):428-35. PubMed ID: 23668696
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 11. Waterbird solves the string-pull test.
    Lamarre J, Wilson DR.
    R Soc Open Sci; 2021 Dec 29; 8(12):211343. PubMed ID: 34966556
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 12. Testing problem solving in turkey vultures (Cathartes aura) using the string-pulling test.
    Ellison AM, Watson J, Demers E.
    Anim Cogn; 2015 Jan 29; 18(1):111-8. PubMed ID: 25015133
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 13. Investigation of cognitive mechanisms and strategy on solving multiple string-pulling problems in Azure-winged magpie (Cyanopica cyanus).
    Wang L, Zhang D, Sui J.
    Anim Cogn; 2021 Jan 29; 24(1):1-10. PubMed ID: 32638171
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 14. An investigation into the cognition behind spontaneous string pulling in New Caledonian crows.
    Taylor AH, Medina FS, Holzhaider JC, Hearne LJ, Hunt GR, Gray RD.
    PLoS One; 2010 Feb 22; 5(2):e9345. PubMed ID: 20179759
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 15. Patterned-string tasks: relation between fine motor skills and visual-spatial abilities in parrots.
    Krasheninnikova A.
    PLoS One; 2013 Feb 22; 8(12):e85499. PubMed ID: 24376885
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 16. Social influences on delayed gratification in New Caledonian crows and Eurasian jays.
    Miller R, Davies JR, Schiestl M, Garcia-Pelegrin E, Gray RD, Taylor AH, Clayton NS.
    PLoS One; 2023 Feb 22; 18(12):e0289197. PubMed ID: 38055711
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 17.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 18. An end to insight? New Caledonian crows can spontaneously solve problems without planning their actions.
    Taylor AH, Knaebe B, Gray RD.
    Proc Biol Sci; 2012 Dec 22; 279(1749):4977-81. PubMed ID: 23097511
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 19. Problem-solving in a cooperative task in peach-fronted conures (Eupsittula aurea).
    Torres Ortiz S, Corregidor Castro A, Balsby TJS, Larsen ON.
    Anim Cogn; 2020 Mar 22; 23(2):265-275. PubMed ID: 31760558
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 20.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]


    Page: [Next] [New Search]
    of 9.