These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


PUBMED FOR HANDHELDS

Journal Abstract Search


207 related items for PubMed ID: 9418226

  • 1.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 2. Apes' and children's understanding of cooperative and competitive motives in a communicative situation.
    Herrmann E, Tomasello M.
    Dev Sci; 2006 Sep; 9(5):518-29. PubMed ID: 16911454
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 3.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 4. Apes' use of iconic cues in the object-choice task.
    Herrmann E, Melis AP, Tomasello M.
    Anim Cogn; 2006 Apr; 9(2):118-30. PubMed ID: 16395566
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 5. The performance of bonobos (Pan paniscus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) in two versions of an object-choice task.
    Mulcahy NJ, Call J.
    J Comp Psychol; 2009 Aug; 123(3):304-9. PubMed ID: 19685972
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 6. Token transfers among great apes (Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus, Pan paniscus, and Pan troglodytes): species differences, gestural requests, and reciprocal exchange.
    Pelé M, Dufour V, Thierry B, Call J.
    J Comp Psychol; 2009 Nov; 123(4):375-84. PubMed ID: 19929106
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 7. Body orientation and face orientation: two factors controlling apes' behavior from humans.
    Kaminski J, Call J, Tomasello M.
    Anim Cogn; 2004 Oct; 7(4):216-23. PubMed ID: 15034765
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 8. A competitive nonverbal false belief task for children and apes.
    Krachun C, Carpenter M, Call J, Tomasello M.
    Dev Sci; 2009 Jul; 12(4):521-35. PubMed ID: 19635080
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 9. Tracking and inferring spatial rotation by children and great apes.
    Okamoto-Barth S, Call J.
    Dev Psychol; 2008 Sep; 44(5):1396-408. PubMed ID: 18793071
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 10. One-year-olds comprehend the communicative intentions behind gestures in a hiding game.
    Behne T, Carpenter M, Tomasello M.
    Dev Sci; 2005 Nov; 8(6):492-9. PubMed ID: 16246240
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 11.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 12.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 13.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 14. How the great apes (Pan troglodytes, Pongo pygmaeus, Pan paniscus, Gorilla gorilla) perform on the reversed reward contingency task II: transfer to new quantities, long-term retention, and the impact of quantity ratios.
    Uher J, Call J.
    J Comp Psychol; 2008 May; 122(2):204-12. PubMed ID: 18489236
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 15.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 16. Does early care affect joint attention in great apes (Pan troglodytes, Pan paniscus, Pongo abelii, Pongo pygmaeus, Gorilla gorilla)?
    Pitman CA, Shumaker RW.
    J Comp Psychol; 2009 Aug; 123(3):334-41. PubMed ID: 19685976
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 17. Tracking the displacement of objects: a series of tasks with great apes (Pan troglodytes, Pan paniscus, Gorilla gorilla, and Pongo pygmaeus) and young children (Homo sapiens).
    Barth J, Call J.
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2006 Jul; 32(3):239-52. PubMed ID: 16834492
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 18. Great apes' strategies to map spatial relations.
    Hribar A, Haun D, Call J.
    Anim Cogn; 2011 Jul; 14(4):511-23. PubMed ID: 21359655
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 19.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 20.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]


    Page: [Next] [New Search]
    of 11.