These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


PUBMED FOR HANDHELDS

Journal Abstract Search


207 related items for PubMed ID: 9680677

  • 1.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 2.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 3. Object permanence in young infants: further evidence.
    Baillargeon R, DeVos J.
    Child Dev; 1991 Dec; 62(6):1227-46. PubMed ID: 1786712
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 4.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 5.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 6.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 7. Auditory-visual intermodal matching of small numerosities in 6-month-old infants.
    Kobayashi T, Hiraki K, Hasegawa T.
    Dev Sci; 2005 Sep; 8(5):409-19. PubMed ID: 16048513
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 8.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 9. Infants interpret ambiguous requests for absent objects.
    Saylor MM, Ganea P.
    Dev Psychol; 2007 May; 43(3):696-704. PubMed ID: 17484581
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 10. The development of perceptual completion abilities: infants' perception of stationary, partially occluded objects.
    Craton LG.
    Child Dev; 1996 Jun; 67(3):890-904. PubMed ID: 8706533
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 11. Detecting continuity violations in infancy: a new account and new evidence from covering and tube events.
    Wang SH, Baillargeon R, Paterson S.
    Cognition; 2005 Mar; 95(2):129-73. PubMed ID: 15694644
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 12.
    ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 13. Infants' understanding of object-directed action.
    Phillips AT, Wellman HM.
    Cognition; 2005 Dec; 98(2):137-55. PubMed ID: 16307956
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 14. Perseverative responding in a violation-of-expectation task in 6.5-month-old infants.
    Aguiar A, Baillargeon R.
    Cognition; 2003 Jul; 88(3):277-316. PubMed ID: 12804814
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 15. What do infants know about continuous quantity?
    Gao F, Levine SC, Huttenlocher J.
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2000 Sep; 77(1):20-9. PubMed ID: 10964457
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 16. The nature and processes of preverbal learning: implications from nine-month-old infants' discrimination problem solving.
    Coldren JT, Colombo J.
    Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 1994 Sep; 59(4):1-75; discussion 76-93. PubMed ID: 7997251
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 17. Conceptual development in infancy: the understanding of containment.
    MacLean DJ, Schuler M.
    Child Dev; 1989 Oct; 60(5):1126-37. PubMed ID: 2805891
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 18. Occluding the trajectory of an object: effects on predicting containment with 9-, 12- and 16-month-olds.
    Dejonckheere PJ, Smitsman AW, Verhofstadt-Denève L.
    Infant Behav Dev; 2007 Aug; 30(3):523-8. PubMed ID: 17683760
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 19. To see or not to see: infants prefer to follow the gaze of a reliable looker.
    Chow V, Poulin-Dubois D, Lewis J.
    Dev Sci; 2008 Sep; 11(5):761-70. PubMed ID: 18801132
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]

  • 20. Make the first move: how infants learn about self-propelled objects.
    Rakison DH.
    Dev Psychol; 2006 Sep; 42(5):900-12. PubMed ID: 16953695
    [Abstract] [Full Text] [Related]


    Page: [Next] [New Search]
    of 11.